Skip to main content
Log in

The hyphen as a syllabification cue in reading bisyllabic and multisyllabic words among Finnish 1st and 2nd graders

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Finnish ABC books present words with hyphens inserted at syllable boundaries. Syllabification by hyphens is abandoned in the 2nd grade for bisyllabic words, but continues for words with three or more syllables. The current eye movement study investigated how and to what extent syllable hyphens in bisyllabic (kah-vi ‘cof-fee’) and multisyllabic words (haa-ruk-ka ‘fork’, ap-pel-sii-ni ‘orange’) affect eye movement behavior and reading speed of Finnish 1st and 2nd graders. Experiment 1 showed that 2nd graders had longer gaze durations, needed more fixations and had longer selective regression path durations for hyphenated than concatenated words. This implies that hyphenated words were difficult to process when first encountered, but also hard to integrate with prior sentence context. The effects were modified by number of syllables and reading skill. That is, the hyphenation effects were larger for multisyllabic than bisyllabic words and larger for more than less proficient readers. Experiment 2 showed the same hyphenation effect for 1st graders reading long multisyllabic words, even with a hyphen that was smaller in size and hence visually less salient. We argue that syllable hyphens prevent reasonably proficient readers from using the most efficient processing route for bi- and multisyllabic words and discuss the possible implications of the results for early Finnish reading instruction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In support of this statement, only 0.1 % of the 70,000 words in the reverse dictionary of modern standard Finnish are monosyllabic (Tuomi, 1980).

  2. A similar slowing down of hyphenation was found by Häikiö, Bertram, and Hyönä (2011) at the morpheme level. They found that compounds with a hyphen at the constituent boundary (e.g., ulko-ovi = ‘front door’) elicited longer reading times for proficient 2nd graders, 4th graders and 6th graders than concatenated compounds (e.g., sivuovi = ‘side door’).

  3. Both of these variables had high correlations with word frequency, so we used their residuals in the models.

  4. Note that exactly the same pattern of results was found for the go-past time, which is the sum of all fixations after entering the target word before exiting it to the right (in Fig. 2 the summed duration of Fixations #3 to #6). For this measure the hyphenation effect was 36 ms for short words and 194 ms for long words.

  5. We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out.

  6. Furthermore, the present results relate to reading of monomorphemic, uninflected words. Since, for example, any Finnish noun can appear in approximately 2,000 possible orthographic forms (Karlsson & Koskenniemi, 1985), most of which have never been encountered before, hyphenation may support the decoding of these typically long morphologically complex words in the early stages of reading development.

References

  • Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4(Rpackageversion1.0+)

  • Ehri, L. C. (1987). Learning to read and spell words. Journal of Reading Behavior, 19, 5–31. doi:10.1080/10862968709547585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability. Journal of Reading Disabilities, 22, 356–365. doi:10.1177/002221948902200606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116–125. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00077.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word learning: Implications for instruction with delayed and disabled readers. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 14, 135–163. doi:10.1080/1057356980140202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). London, UK: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., Lété, B., Bertand, D., Sufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition, 123, 280–292. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 54. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Häikiö, T., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2011). The development of whole-word representations in compound word processing: Evidence from eye fixation patterns of elementary school children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 533–551. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Häikiö, T., Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2015). The role of syllables in word recognition among beginning Finnish readers: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27, 562–577. doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.982126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyönä, J., & Olson, R. K. (1995). Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1430–1440. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.6.1430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karlsson, F., & Koskenniemi, K. (1985). A process model of morphology and lexicon. Folia Linguistica, 29, 207–231. doi:10.1515/flin.1985.19.1-2.207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karppi, S. (1983). Lukutaidon ABC. Johdatus lukemisen ja kirjoittamisen perustekniikan opetukseen. [The ABC of reading skill. Introduction to teaching the basic techniques of reading and writing.] Espoo, Finland: Weilin + Göös.

  • Laine, M., & Virtanen, P. (1999). WordMill lexical search program. Turku, Finland: University of Turku, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerkkanen, M.-K. (2006). Lukemaan oppiminen ja opettaminen esi- ja alkuopetuksessa. [Learning and teaching reading in preschool and early education.] Helsinki, Finland: WSOY Oppimateriaalit.

  • Lindeman, J. (1998). ALLU: Ala-asteen Lukutesti [ALLU: Reading test for elementary school]. Turku, Finland: University of Turku, Center for Learning Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria; http://www.R-project.org

  • Salmi, P., Eklund, K., Järvisalo, E., & Aro, M. (2011). LukiMatOppimisen arviointi: Lukemisen ja kirjoittamisen tuen tarpeen tunnistamisen välineet 1. luokalle. [LukiMatevaluating learning: Tools for recognizing the need of support in reading and writing for the 1st grade.] Jyväskylä, Finland: Niilo Mäki Instituutti.

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuomi, T. (1980). Suomen kielen käänteissanakirja [Reverse dictionary of modern standard Finnish] (2nd ed.). Hämeenlinna, Finland: Finnish Literature Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wäre, M., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Suonranta-Hollo, L., Korolainen, T., Parkkinen, J., Kirkkopelto, K., & Ketonen, R. (2014). Pikkumetsän aapinen [ABC book of Tiny Forest]. Helsinki, Finland: Sanoma Pro.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was financially supported by the Finnish Graduate School of Psychology, Oskar Öflunds Stiftelse [Oskar Öflund Foundation] and Suomen Kulttuurirahaston Varsinais-Suomen rahasto [Finnish Culture Foundation, Varsinais-Suomi Regional Fund] to the first author and the Academy of Finland (Grant 118404) to the second author. We would like to thank Petar Milin for an advice on an earlier version of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tuomo Häikiö.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Final models for each measure in the Experiment 1 with fixed effects presented for each measure separately. If an interaction was significant, its main effects are reported as well. Please note that in this case the main effects are not independently interpretable in the lmer() output. Also note that due to the transformations the directions of the effects are reversed. An effect is significant when |t| > 2.

Tables 5, 6, 7 and 8.

Table 5 First fixation duration: fixed effects
Table 6 Gaze duration: fixed effects
Table 7 Number of fixations: fixed effects
Table 8 Selective regression path duration: fixed effects

Appendix 2

Final models for each measure in the Experiment 2a with fixed effects presented for each measure separately. If an interaction was significant, its main effects are reported as well. Please note that in this case the main effects are not independently interpretable in the lmer() output. Also note that due to the transformations the directions of the effects are reversed for First Fixation Duration. An effect is significant when |t| > 2.

Tables 9, 10, 11 and 12.

Table 9 First fixation duration: fixed effects
Table 10 Gaze duration: fixed effects
Table 11 Number of fixations: fixed effects
Table 12 Selective regression path duration: fixed effects

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Häikiö, T., Bertram, R. & Hyönä, J. The hyphen as a syllabification cue in reading bisyllabic and multisyllabic words among Finnish 1st and 2nd graders. Read Writ 29, 159–182 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9584-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9584-x

Keywords

Navigation