Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Reading in two orthographies: A cross-linguistic study of Dutch average and poor readers who learn English as a second language

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated the reading of secondary school students in their first and second language (L1, L2). Twenty-six average and twenty-six poor readers matched on age, gender, listening and reading comprehension participated. They were native Dutch speakers who started learning English at secondary school (grade 7). We examined whether differences in L2 between the two groups reflect differences in L1 with regard to reading and relevant subskills. In addition, the relationship between reading and its predictors within and across the two languages was investigated. Between group differences were similar in L1 and L2 when task conditions involved high levels of phonological and orthographic complexity or demanded speeded processing. Furthermore, serial rapid naming predicted speeded word reading in both languages and L2 text reading accuracy, while L2 phoneme awareness and orthographic knowledge explained unique variance in L2 text reading accuracy. Cross-linguistic prediction revealed that speeded word reading predicted its counterpart from L1 to L2 and vice versa. Serial rapid naming explained additional variance in the prediction of L2 from L1. After exclusion of the reading predictor from the model, serial rapid naming was the most consistent cross-linguistic predictor, while L2 orthographic knowledge explained a small amount of unique variance in L1 speeded word reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Badian N. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction Annals of Dyslexia 51: 179–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey J. A., McGuigan M., Ruschena A. (2005). On the associations between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account Journal of Research in Reading 28: 400–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits Developmental Psychology 28(5): 874–885

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brus B. T., Voeten M. J. M. (1973). Een-minuut-test [One-minute-test] Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Berkhout B. V

    Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas M., Volin J., Hulme C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 92: 107–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins C., Pennington B. (2004). The relationship between phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: The role of developmental period and reading ability Scientific Studies of Reading 8: 28–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisero C., Royer J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness Contemporary Educational Psychology 20: 275–303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J. (1988). Statistical power analysis of the behavioral science (2nd ed) New York: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Comeau L., Cormier P., Grandmaison E., Lacroix D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language Journal of Educational Psychology 9(1): 29–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton D. (2000). Modeling the growth of decoding skills in first grade children Scientific Studies of Reading 4: 219–258

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cossu G. (1999). Biological constraints on literacy acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 11: 213–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham A., Perry K., Stanovich K. (2001). Converging evidence for the concept of orthographic processing Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 14: 549–568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham A., Perry K., Stanovich K., Share D. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 82: 185–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham A., Stanovich K. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience Journal of Educational Psychology 84(4): 733–740

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong P. F., van der Leij A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study Journal of Educational Psychology 91(3): 450–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong P. F., van der Leij A. (2003). Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography Journal of Educational Psychology 95(1): 22–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla M. B., Rudel R. G. (1974). ‘Rapid automatised naming’ of pictured objects, colours, letters and numbers by normal children Cortex 10: 186–202

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn L. M., Dunn L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu A. Y., Nagy W., Hancin-Bhatt B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness Journal of Educational Psychology 85(3): 543–565

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 1–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro C., Borstrom I., Petersen D. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctiveness of phonological representations of lexical items Reading Research Quarterly 33(1): 36–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro C., Nielsen I., Petersen D. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items Annals of Dyslexia 44: 205–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott C. D. (1983). British Ability Scales, manual 2, technical handbook Windsor: NFER-Nelson

    Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett A. J., Nicolson R. I. (1996). The Dyslexia Screening Test London: The Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton R., Brown I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 39–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman B. R., Francis D. J. (1994). Exploring connections among reading, spelling, and phonemic segmentation during first grade Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6: 65–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman B. F., Francis D. J., Fletcher J. M., Lynn A. (1996). Relation of phonological and orthographic processing to early reading: Comparing two approaches to regression-based, reading-level-match designs Journal of Educational Psychology 88(4): 639–652

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In: Brady S. A., Shankweiler D. P. (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 97–117)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher A. M., Laxon V., Armstrong E., Frith U. (1996). Phonological difficulties in high functioning dyslexics Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 499–509

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow L., Sparks R., Javorsky J., Pohlman J., Bishop-Marbury A. (1991). Identifying native language difficulties among foreign language learners in college: A ‘foreign’ language learning disability? Journal of Learning Disabilities 24(9): 530–541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva E., Siegel L. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages Reading and Writing 12: 1–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1999). Towards a theoretical framework for understanding reading development and dyslexia in different orthographies. In I. Lundberg, F. E. Tonnesen, & I. Austad (Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and practice. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers

  • Gottardo A., Yan B., Siegel L. S., Wade-Wooley L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing Journal of Educational Psychology 93(3): 530–542

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacquebord I. (1992). ‘Alle leerlingen’. Toets voor begrijpend lezen. [‘All students’. Test for reading comprehension] Groningen: Universiteit van Groningen

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris M., Hatano G. (1999). Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho C. S., Lai D. N. (2000). Naming speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia Learning and Individual Differences 11: 173–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuijpers C., van der Leij A., Been P., van Leeuwen T., ter Keurs M., Schreuder R., van den Bos K. P. (2003). Leesproblemen in het voortgezet onderwijs en de volwassenheid [Reading problems in secondary school and adulthood] Pedagogische Studiën 80(4): 272–287

    Google Scholar 

  • Landerl K., Wimmer H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children Applied Psycholinguistics 21: 243–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl K., Wimmer H., Frith U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison Cognition 63: 315–334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsey K. A., Manis F. R., Bailey C. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners Journal of Educational Psychology 95(3): 482–494

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg I., Frost J., Petersen O. P. (1988). Reading and spelling skills the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness in kindergarten Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 21: 159–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis F. R., Custodio R., Szeszulski P. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 56: 64–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis F. R., Seidenberg M. S., Doi L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders Scientific Studies of Reading 3: 129–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang C., Manis F., Seidenberg M., Custodio R., Doi L. (1993). Print exposure as a predictor of word reading and reading comprehension in disabled and nondisabled readers Journal of Educational Psychology 85(2): 230–238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller-Guron L., Lundberg I. (2000). Dyslexia and second language reading: A second bite at the apple?Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 12: 41–61

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter V., Hulme C., Snowling M., Taylor S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming predicts early progress in learning to read Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 65: 370–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neale M. D., Whetton C., Caspall L., McCulloch K. (1997). Neale Analysis of Reading Ability-Revised. Manual for psychological services Windsor: Whurr

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson R., Forsberg H., Wise B., Rack J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic, and phonological skills. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the learning disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson R. K., Wise B., Conners F., Rack J., Fulker D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences Journal of Learning Disabilities 22: 339–348

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel T. K., Snowling M., de Jong P. F. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and Dutch Journal of Educational Psychology 96: 785–797

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A. (1985). Reading ability New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In: P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp.145–174). City, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Porpodas C. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek Journal of Learning Disabilities 32(5): 406–416

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack J. P., Snowling M. J., Olson R. K. (1992). The non-word reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review Reading Research Quarterly 27(1): 29–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma P. (1983). Word-specific knowledge in beginning reading Journal of Research in Reading 6: 41–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riccio C. A., Amado A., Jimenez S., Hasbrouck J. E., Imhoff B., Denton C. (2001). Cross-linguistic transfer of phonological processing: Development of a measure of phonological processing in Spanish Bilingual Research Journal 25(4): 416–436

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour P. (1986). Cognitive analysis of dyslexia London: Routledge

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour P., Aro M., Erskine J. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies British Journal of Psychology 94: 143–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share D. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition Cognition 55: 151–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share D. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72: 95–129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel L. S., Share D., Geva E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics Psychological Science 6(4): 250–254

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling M., Goulandris N., Defty N. (1996). A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 653–669

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spring C., Davis J. (1988). Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading Applied Psycholinguistics 9: 315–344

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich K., West R. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing Reading Research Quarterly 24(4): 402–427

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike E. L., Lorge I. (1944). The teacher’s word book of 30000 words New York: Teachers College

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. A., Alexander A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationship with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency and the brain (333–355). Parkton, MD: York Press

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos K. P. (1996). BELL 96 The Netherlands: Department of Special Education, Groningen University

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos K. P.. (1998). IQ, phonological awareness and continuous-naming speed related to Dutch poor decoding children’s performance on two word identification tests Dyslexia 4: 73–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos K. P., Spelberg H. C., Scheepstra A. J. M., de Vries J. (1994). De Klepel. Vorm A en B. Een test voor de leesvaardigheid van pseudowoorden. Verantwoording, handleiding, diagnostiek en behandeling. [The Klepel. Form A and B. A test of reading pseudowords]. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Berkhout

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Bosch K., van Bon W. H. J., Schreuder R. (1995). Poor readers’ decoding skills: Effects of training with limited exposure duration Reading Research Quarterly 30: 110–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos K. P., Zijlstra B. J., Lutje Spelberg H. C. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colours, pictured objects and word-reading speed Scientific Studies of Reading 6: 25–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Leij A., Morfidi E. (2006) Core deficits and variable differences in Dutch poor readers learning English Journal of Learning Disabilities 39: 74–90

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Leij A., van Daal V. (1999). Automatization aspects of dyslexia: Speed limitation in word identification, sensitivity to increasing task demands, and orthographic compensation Journal of Learning Disabilities 32(5): 417–428

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F. R., Fletcher J. M., Snowling M. J., Scanlon D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 45: 2–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F. R., Scanlon D. M. (1986). Linguistic coding and metalinguistic awareness: Their relationship to verbal memory and code acquisition in poor and normal readers. In D. B. Yaden, S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning reading (pp. 115–141). NH: Heinemann Portsmouth

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F. R., Scanlon D. M., Tanzman M. S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co

    Google Scholar 

  • Wade-Wooley L., Geva E. (2000). Processing novel phonemic contrasts in acquisition of L2 word reading Scientific Studies of Reading 4(4): 295–311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner R. K., Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. A.. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study Developmental Psychology 30: 73–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner R. K., Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. A. Hecht S. A., Barker T. A., Burgess S. R. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled reader: A 5-year longitudinal study Developmental Psychology 33: 468–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willems M. M., Oud-de Glas M. M. B. (1990). Vocabulaire-selectie voor het vreemde-talenonderwijs [Vocabulary selection for foreign language education] Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Instituut voor Toegepaste Sociale Wetenschappen

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system Applied Psycholinguistics 14: 1–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H. (1996). The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 1–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H., Mayringer H., Landerl K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography Journal of Educational Psychology 92(4): 668–680

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M., Bally H., Morris R. (1986). Towards a more universal understanding of the developmental dyslexias: The contribution of orthographic factors In: Berninger V. W. (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M., Bowers P. G. (1999). The double deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 415–438

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yap R., van der Leij A. (1993). Word processing in dyslexics: An automatic decoding deficit? Reading and Writing 5: 261–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler J. C., Goswami U. (2005). Reading acquisition developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory Psychological Bulletin 131(1): 3–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Hieke Wagenaar for her assistance in data collection and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. This research was financed by a grant from the European ‘Human Potential’ programme (HPMF-CT-2001-01438) to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eleni Morfidi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Morfidi, E., van der Leij, A., de Jong, P.F. et al. Reading in two orthographies: A cross-linguistic study of Dutch average and poor readers who learn English as a second language. Read Writ 20, 753–784 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9035-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9035-9

Keywords

Navigation