Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

It can happen here: the impact of the Mumbai terror attacks on public opinion in Western Europe

  • Published:
Public Choice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Do terror attacks have an impact on public opinion, even if the terror attacks happen far away? We exploit the fact that the fourth round of the European Social Survey was conducted in several West European countries at the time of the 2008 terror attacks in Mumbai, India, in order to identify the causal effect of the Mumbai attacks on public opinion. We identify a clear jump in fear of terrorism at home as a consequence of the terror attacks, but despite the increase in fear of terrorism, we find no significant effect of the attack on support for illiberal interrogation techniques or for liberal immigration policies. We do find indications of a shift in conservative direction on the left–right scale, but this shift is not significant in all time windows. Our findings suggest that a terror attack needs to have a very large impact on the fear of terrorism before people change their policy preferences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. A.1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. But see Frey et al. (2009), who study how terrorism affects survey respondents’ reported life satisfaction.

  2. For instance, Becker and Rubinstein (2010) argue that some people will invest effort to reduce the discrepancy between their subjective beliefs about the likelihood of terrorism and the objective danger, rather than changing the demand for a “terror-infected” good—the “terror-infected” good in our case being civil liberty.

  3. The Madrid attack took place only 3 days before the parliamentary election, an election where the incumbent conservative government suffered heavy defeats. Thus, the conservative shift on the left–right scale appears to have been completely reversed by the conservative government’s handling of the attack (see Montalvo 2011, for an analysis of the electoral impact of the attack).

  4. The procedures for assigning interview dates vary somewhat across countries, but the process is generally as follows: the interviewers are allotted a number of sampled units with whom they attempt to make appointments, either face-to-face or by phone (but phone calls to make appointments are acceptable only if the country can provide evidence that the response rate will not be damaged by appointments being made by phone rather than face-to-face). At least four unsuccessful personal visits must be attempted before the sampled unit is abandoned. All interviews have to be made face-to-face. One of the most important goals is to achieve a high response rate and much of the work is tailored to obtain this goal. We find it unlikely that the procedures for obtaining a high response rate should systematically influence our results. See www.europeansocialsurvey.org for detailed fieldwork and data documentation.

  5. We control for gender, age, income, education level, whether the respondent is born in the respective country, and whether the respondent lives in an urban area. Strictly speaking, we do not need to include all of these controls since there is a significant difference in means between treatment and control groups only for age (see Table A.1). However, including exogenous controls might increase precision, but this is not the case here, possibly because the number of observations is reduced when we include controls.

  6. We chose 2 months later rather than 1 month later because 1 month later is in the middle of the Christmas vacation, a period during which fewer respondents were interviewed.

  7. Multilevel modeling is especially useful when one wants to examine variation across groups, as the estimates of country-specific coefficients represent a compromise between estimating separate coefficients for each country and one coefficient for all countries (see Gelman and Hill 2007 for an introduction to multilevel modeling). In practice, this implies that the country-specific estimates for those coefficients that are allowed to vary across countries represent a compromise between the mean across countries and the country-specific data. This compromise is guided by the sample size so that the estimates for countries with small samples are driven more by the cross-country relationship.

  8. We estimate linear models since we find that the estimated cross-national variation in the treatment effect estimated by the multilevel linear probability model appears more plausible in light of the descriptive data. It is well known that the multilevel logit/probit model is less accurate than the linear multilevel model when the sample size at level two is fairly small (see, for example, Hox 2002: 176). We would like to stress, however, that the multilevel probit’s estimate of the average (cross-national) treatment effect is identical to the estimate from the multilevel linear model, and that all conclusions from the single-level models presented earlier in the paper are similar whether we estimate logit, probit or linear probability models. The only apparent problem with the probit model is its estimate of the degree of cross-national variation in the multilevel models.

References

  • Angrist, J. D., & Pischke, J.-S. (2009). Mostly harmless econometrics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Dai, B. (2008). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-28. URL: http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/.

  • Becker, G., & Rubinstein, Y. (2010). Fear and response to terrorism: an economic analysis. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Berrebi, C., & Klor, E. F. (2008). Are voters sensitive to terrorism? Direct evidence from the Israeli electorate. American Political Science Review, 102(3), 279–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blekesaune, M. (2007). Economic conditions and public attitudes to welfare policies. European Sociological Review, 23(3), 393–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bozzoli, C., & Müller, C. (2009). Perceptions and attitudes to a terrorist shock: evidence from the UK (Economics of Security Working Paper 13).

  • Davis, D. W., & Silver, B. D. (2004). Civil liberties vs. security: public opinion in the context of the terrorist attacks on America. American Journal of Political Science, 48(1), 28–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Echebarria-Echabe, A., & Fernández-Guede, E. (2006). Effects of terrorism on attitudes and ideological orientation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(2), 259–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eggers, A. C., & Hainmueller, J. (2009). MPs for sale? Returns to office in postwar British politics. American Political Science Review, 103(4), 513–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finseraas, H., Jakobsson, N., & Kotsadam, A. (2011). Did the murder of Theo van Gogh change Europeans’ immigration policy preferences? Kyklos, 64(3), 396–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B., Leuchinger, S. & Stutzer, A. (2009). The life satisfaction approach to valuing public goods: the case of terrorism. Public Choice, 138(3–4), 317–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, D., & Klingemann, H.-D. (1990). The left-right schema. In K. M. Jennings & J. W. van Deth (Eds.), Continuities in political action (pp. 203–234). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, E. D., & Klor, E. F. (2010). Does terrorism work? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 125(4), 1459–1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, A., & Rajiv, S. S. C. (2008). Terror strikes Mumbai: the world reacts. Global media responses to the Mumbai blasts: editorials, op-eds, commentaries, and articles in leading newspapers. Institute for Defense Studies and Analyses, New Dehli, India.

  • Hainmueller, J., & Kern, H. L. (2008). Incumbency as a source of spillover effects in mixed electoral systems: Evidence from a regression-discontinuity design. Electoral Studies, 27(2), 213–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M. J., & Suhay, E. (2011). Authoritarianism, threat, and Americans’ support for the war on terror. American Journal of Political Science. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00514.x.

  • Hox, J. (2002 ). Multilevel analysis: techniques and applications. Mahwah: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, anxiety, and support for antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 593–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kastellec, J. P., & Leoni, E. L. (2007). Using graphs instead of tables in political science. Perspectives on Politics, 5(4), 755–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurrild-Klitgaard, P., Justesen M. K., & Klemmensen R. (2006). The political economy of freedom, democracy and transnational terrorism. Public Choice, 128(1–2), 7–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krieger, T., & Meierrieks, D. (2011). What causes terrorism? Public Choice, 147(1–2), 3–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A., & Kinder, D. R. (1990). Altering the foundations for support for the president through priming. American Political Science Review, 84(2), 497–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. S., Moretti, E., & Butler, M. J. (2004). Do Voters affect or elect policies? Evidence from the US House. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119(3), 807–859.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2002). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: a national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, S. J., & Freese, J. (2006). Regression models for categorical dependent variables using Stata. College Station: Stata Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montalvo, J. G. (2011). Voting after the bombings: a natural experiment on the effect of terror attacks on democratic elections. Review of Economics and Statistics. doi:10.1162/REST_a_00115.

  • Morella, J. L., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2009). Democracy at risk: how terrorist threats affect the public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shughart, W. F. (2006). An analytical history of terrorism, 1945–2000. Public Choice, 128(1–2), 7–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shughart, W. F. (2011). Terrorism in rational choice perspective. In C. J. Coyne & R. L. Mathers (Eds.), The handbook of the political economy of war. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Andreas Kotsadam, Niklas Jakobsson, Bjarne Strøm, and participants at the Political Behavior Seminar at the Institute for Social Research (ISF) for useful comments. The usual disclaimer applies.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Henning Finseraas.

Appendix

Appendix

Table A.1 Background characteristics (independent variables) and treatment status (dependent variable). Marginal effects after probit
Table A.2 Terror attack in Mumbai, India, and fear of terror attack in own country. Marginal effects after probit
Table A.3 Terror attack in Mumbai, India, and fear of terror attack in own country. Marginal effects after probit
Table A.4 Descriptive statistics for fear of terror by country

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Finseraas, H., Listhaug, O. It can happen here: the impact of the Mumbai terror attacks on public opinion in Western Europe. Public Choice 156, 213–228 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11127-011-9895-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11127-011-9895-7

Keywords

Navigation