Skip to main content
Log in

Bending perception to desire: Effects of task demands, motivation, and cognitive resources

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Determinants of motivated judgments were examined in this research. Three experiments investigated how dominant motivation, biasing difficulty and mental resources combine to produce motivationally congruent judgments. Studies 1 and 2 showed that where a biasing motivation is dominant the presence of resources can augment a motivational bias in judgment. Study 3 replicated that result and showed that resources contribute to the formation of biased judgments only where biasing is difficult to accomplish, but not where it is relatively easy to accomplish. In addition, Study 3 showed that where the accuracy motivation is dominant and biasing is the easy default, unbiased judgments will occur only in the presence (vs. absence) of resources. In contrast, where unbiased judgments are easy to come by, such judgments occur irrespective of resources.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Although we hypothesized two different result patterns between accuracy and biasing motivation conditions, the expected patterns were not expected to be necessarily detected by a four-way interaction. This is so because several of the predicted effects were expected to be in the same direction in both the accuracy and the biasing motivation condition, and in the match versus mismatch conditions. Specifically, some directional tendency toward biased judgments (claiming the ownership for positive traits and disowning negative ones) was assumed to be present in both the biasing motivation and the accuracy motivation condition, though it was expected to be more pronounced in the former versus the latter condition. Similarly, the same direction of bias (toward self enhancement) should be expected for the ambiguous and the unambiguous traits, even though the latter should be more amenable to bias [as Dunning et al. (1989) demonstrated]. The only expected differences between conditions were that in the biasing motivation condition the biasing tendency should have been enhanced in the match-unambiguous traits, whereas in the accuracy condition, the biasing tendency should have been enhanced in the mismatch-ambiguous traits. These subtle differences may be readily swamped in the four way interaction by the omnibus tendencies to perceive the positive versus negative traits as more characteristic of self, and by the two way interaction that shows that difference to be more pronounced for ambiguous versus unambiguous traits.

References

  • Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J., Newcomb, T. M., Rosenberg, M. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1968). Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook. Chicago: Rand-McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumhart, R. (1968). An honest profit. New York: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Science, 1, 319–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of general Psychology, 3, 23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Schurtz, D. R., & Gailliot, M. T. (2010). Acting on limited resources: The interactive effects of self-regulatory depletion and individual differences, Chapter 11. In R. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of Personality and Self-Regulation.

  • Ditto, P. H., Scepansky, J. A., Munro, G. D., Apanovitch, A. M., & Lockhart, L. K. (1998). Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 53.

  • Dunning, D. (1999). A newer look: Motivated social cognition and the schematic representation of social concepts. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1082.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford (1920) concept of defense mechanisms University Press.

  • Freud, S. (1920). A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York, NY, US: Horace Liveright.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Freund, T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Shpitzajzen, A. (1985). The freezing and unfreezing of impressional primacy effects of the need for structure and the fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 479–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K. (1996). Elastic justification: How unjustifiable factors influence judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(1), 122–129.

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press.

  • Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and attribution process. In E. E. Jones, et al. (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 151–174). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. C., Schmitz, F., Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Voss, A. (2010). Understanding the role of executive control in the Implicit Association Test: Why flexible people have small IAT effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 595–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knowles, M. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2008). Benefits of membership: The activation and amplification of group identities in response to social rejection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1200–1213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krizan, Z., & Windschitl, P. D. (2007). The influence of outcome desirability on optimism. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 95–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Bélanger, J. J., Chen, X., Köpetz, C., Pierro, A., & Mannetti, L. (2012). The energetics of motivated cognition: A force-field analysis. Psychological Review, 119, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 448–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Köpetz, C., Bélanger, J. J., Chun, W. Y., Orehek, E., & Fishbach, A. (2013). Features of multifinality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 22–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A., & Pierro, A. (2008). On circadian mismatches and transference effect in social perception. Psychological Science, 19(3), 296–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 331–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z., & Sanitioso, R. (1989). Motivated changes in the self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25(3), 272–285.

  • Kunda, Z., & Sinclair, L. (1999). Motivated reasoning with stereotypes: Activation, application, and inhibition. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 12–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2005). Exclusion and nonconscious behavioral mimicry. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 279–295). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larwood, L. (1978). Swine flu: A field study of self-serving biases. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 283–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, K. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1999). Resisting stereotype change: The role of motivation and attentional capacity in defending social beliefs. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 2, 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, W. N., & Reilly, N. P. (1987). Toward the self-regulation of mood: Theory and research. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 215–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 894–906.

  • Pareto, V. (1916). The mind and society (1st Italian edition) (Translated. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1935).

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 1123–1205). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickett, C. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2005). The social monitoring system: Enhanced sensitivity to social cues and information as an adaptive response to social exclusion and belonging need. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 213–226). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. S., Reilly, C., & Midkiff, K. (1989). Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 728–738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychologica, 47, 143–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Niedenthal, P. M., Covert, M. V., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Are shame and guilt related to distinct self-discrepancies? A test of Higgins’s (1987) hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A. (1976). Attitude polarization as a function of thought and reality constraints. Journal of Research in Personality, 10, 183–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and the perseverance of first impressions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 285–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1985). Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 227–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1093–1104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (1998). Ability perception and cardiovascular response to behavioral challenge. In Personal control in action (pp. 197–232). US: Springer.

  • Wright, R. A. (2008). Refining the prediction of effort: Brehm’s distinction between potential motivation and motivation intensity. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(2), 682–701.

  • Yzerbyt, V. Y., Coull, A., & Rocher, S. J. (1999). Fencing off the deviant: The role of cognitive resources in the maintenance of stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 449–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by a grant from the Social Sciences Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to the first author and an NSF Grant 0314291/0313483 to the second author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jocelyn J. Bélanger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bélanger, J.J., Kruglanski, A.W., Chen, X. et al. Bending perception to desire: Effects of task demands, motivation, and cognitive resources. Motiv Emot 38, 802–814 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9436-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9436-z

Keywords

Navigation