Skip to main content
Log in

Antecedents of everyday positive emotions: An experience sampling analysis

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The focus of this study is on everyday positive emotions and their relations to critical appraisal antecedents. Following from classical appraisal theory and Pekrun’s (2006) control-value theory of achievement emotions, two research questions were addressed, namely whether cognitive appraisals of control and value were related to discrete positive emotions in everyday situations and whether control and value antecedents interact in predicting these emotions. We further investigated whether control/value and positive emotion relations changed as a function of situational factors (achievement vs. non-achievement settings). 50 university freshmen (78% female) were assessed by use of the experience sampling method for a period of 1 week, with intraindividual analyses conducted using a multilevel, idiographic approach. Consistent with our hypotheses, the emotions of enjoyment, pride, and contentment were positively related to control and value appraisals. Further, control and value interacted to predict these positive emotions. The strength of appraisal/positive emotion relations was equivalent across achievement vs. non-achievement settings. Implications for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. E.g., “Men are disturbed not by the things which happen, but by their opinions about the things” (Epictetus; Long 1991, p. 14).

  2. An additional brief report by Tong et al. (2005) is not presented due to including a subset of variables subsequently evaluated in Tong et al. (2007).

  3. Very few participants’ reports (1.8% of 2,026 fully or partially completed questionnaires) referred to situations that were not learning or achievement-related in the classical sense (e.g., studying), but may have been perceived by the individual as achievement-oriented in nature (e.g., sports, playing an instrument, playing chess). However, to avoid potential confounds involving the leisure-oriented nature of such activities, they were coded as non-achievement situations in the present analyses.

  4. In the present study, multiple constructs were assessed using single items (cf., Goetz et al. 2007; Wanous et al. 1997). Although multi-item measures are generally preferable for assessing multi-faceted constructs, such measures take longer to complete than single-item measures and could compromise the assessment of state emotional experiences. More specifically, when participants are asked about state emotions, longer self-report measures could confound participants’ responses by introducing greater reflection time and recall bias. In addition, by taking more time to respond, multi-item scales might be in danger of assessing participants’ emotions with respect to completing the questionnaire, rather than emotions concerning the activity they are currently engaged in. For these reasons, single-item measures were assumed to be more appropriate than multi-item measures for use in this study.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Clements, C. M. (1992). Illusion of control: Invulnerability to negative affect and depressive symptoms after laboratory and natural stressors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 234–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy. The experience of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergin, D. A. (1999). Influences on classroom interest. Educational Psychologist, 34, 87–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M. (1989). Negative reactions to increases in perceived personal control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 246–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, M. J., Finkelstein, L. M., & Dusig, M. S. (1999). On average deviation indices for estimating interrater agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 2(1), 49–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions require cognition. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 181–191). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Covington, M. V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 9, 526–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foersterling, F. (1985). Attributional retraining: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 495–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foersterling, F. (1986). Attributional conceptions in clinical psychology. American Psychologist, 41, 275–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, B. J., & Walker, S. L. (2005). Development and validation of an instrument for assessing distance education learning environments in higher education: The distance education learning environments survey (DELES). Learning Environments Research, 8, 289–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Lüdtke, O., Pekrun, R., & Sutton, R. E. (2009). Emotional transmission in the classroom: Exploring the relationship between teacher and student enjoyment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 705–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2007a). Girls and mathematics—A “hopeless” issue? A control-value approach to gender differences in emotions towards mathematics. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22(4), 497–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frenzel, A. C., Thrash, T. M., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2007b). Achievement Emotions in Germany and China: A cross-cultural validation of the academic emotions questionnaire-mathematics (AEQ-M). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(3), 302–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T. (2004). Emotionales Erleben und selbstreguliertes Lernen bei Schülern im Fach Mathematik [Students’ emotions and self-regulated learning in mathematics]. Munich: Utz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, C. A., Hall, N. C., & Pekrun, R. (2008). Antecedents of academic emotions: Testing the internal/external frame of reference model for academic enjoyment. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 9–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, C. A., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Lüdtke, O. (2007a). Between- and within-domain relations of students’ academic emotions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 715–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Haag, L. (2006). Academic emotions from a social-cognitive perspective: Antecedents and domain specificity of students’ affect in the context of Latin instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 289–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Preckel, F., Pekrun, R., & Hall, N. C. (2007b). Emotional experiences during test taking: Does cognitive ability make a difference? Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, N. C., Perry, R. P., Chipperfield, J. G., Clifton, R. A., & Haynes, T. L. (2006). Enhancing primary and secondary control in achievement settings through writing-based attributional retraining. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25, 361–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, N. C., Perry, R. P., Goetz, T., Ruthig, J. C., Stupnisky, R. H., & Newall, N. E. (2007). Attributional retraining and elaborative learning: Improving academic development through writing-based interventions. Learning and Individual Differences, 17(3), 280–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 283–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience sampling method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henson, H. N., & Chang, E. C. (1998). Locus of control and the fundamental dimensions of moods. Psychological Reports, 82, 1335–1338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, E. H. (1994). Becoming literate through authentic tasks: Evidence and adaptations. In H. Singer, R. B. Ruddell, & M. R. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffart, A., & Martisen, E. W. (1990). Agoraphobia, depression, mental health locus of control, and attributional styles. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 343–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamiell, J. T. (1998). ‘Nomothetic’ and ‘idiographic’: Contrasting Windelband’s understanding with contemporary usage. Theory & Psychology, 8(1), 23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaption. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1995). Vexing research problems inherent in cognitive-mediational theories of emotion and some solutions. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 183–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lefcourt, H. M. (Ed.). (1983). Research with the locus of control construct: Vol 2. Developments and social problems. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, H. (1973). Perceived parental antecedents of internal, powerful others, and chance locus of control orientations. Developmental Psychology, 9, 260–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, G. (1991). Epictetus: The Enchiridion (chapter 5). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luedtke, O., Trautwein, U., Kunter, M., & Baumert, J. (2006). Reliability and agreement of student ratings of the classroom environment: A reanalysis of TIMSS data. Learning Environments Research, 9, 215–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., & Ayotte, V. (2003). Do multiple dimensions of self-concept become more differentiated with age? The differential distinctiveness hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 687–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., & Rowe, K. J. (1996). The negative effects of school-average ability on academic self-concept: An application of multilevel modelling. Australian Journal of Education, 40, 65–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nezlek, J. B., Vansteelandt, K., Mechelen, I. V., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Appraisal-emotion relationships in daily life. Emotion, 8(1), 145–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R. (2000). A social-cognitive, control-value theory of achievement emotions. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development (pp. 143–163). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 18, 315–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2009). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring appraisal antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology (in press).

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002a). Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37(2), 91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002b). Positive emotions in education. In E. Frydenberg (Ed.), Beyond coping: Meeting goals, visions, and challenges (pp. 149–174). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, R. P., Hall, N. C., & Ruthig, J. C. (2007). Perceived (academic) control and scholastic attainment in college students. In R. P. Perry & J. Smart (Eds.), The scholarship of teaching and learning in higher education: An evidence-based perspective (pp. 477–551). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models. Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A., & Congdon, R. (2007). HLM 6.04. Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood: Scientific Software International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J. (2001). A model of appraisal in the emotion system: Integrating theory, research and applications. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion (pp. 68–91). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 241–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J., & Smith, C. A. (2001). Appraisal theory: Overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion (pp. 3–19). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J., Spindel, M. S., & Jose, P. E. (1990). Appraisals of emotion-eliciting events: Testing a theory of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 899–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rost, D. H., & Schermer, F. J. (1987). Emotion and cognition in coping with test anxiety. Communication and Cognition, 20, 225–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1) (Whole No. 609).

  • Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (Ed.). (1988). Facets of emotion. Recent research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion (pp. 92–120). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Wranik, T., Sangsue, J., Tran, V., & Scherer, U. (2004). Emotions in everyday life: Probability of risk factors, appraisal and reaction patterns. Social Science Information, 43, 499–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55(1), 5–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A. (1985). Action, control judgments, and the structure of control experience. Psychological Review, 92, 39–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3), 549–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for the structure of coping: A review and critique of category systems for classifying ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 216–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., Haynes, K. N., Lazarus, R. S., & Pope, L. K. (1993). In search of the “hot” cognitions: Attributions, appraisals, and their relations to emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 916–929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spacks, P. M. (1995). Boredom: The literary history of a state of mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stumpf, C. (1899). Über den Begriff der Gemüthsbewegung [On the concept of emotion]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 44, 1–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tong, E. M. W., Bishop, G. D., Enkelmann, H. C., Why, Y. P., Diong, S. M., Khader, M., et al. (2005). The use of ecological momentary assessment to test appraisal theories of emotion. Emotion, 5(4), 508–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, E. M. W., Bishop, G. D., Enkelmann, H. C., Why, Y. P., Diong, S. M., Khader, M., et al. (2007). Emotion and appraisal: A study using ecological momentary assessment. Cognition and Emotion, 21(7), 1361–1381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: How good are single-item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(2), 247–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeidner, M., & Endler, N. (Eds.). (1996). Handbook of coping. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Goetz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Goetz, T., Frenzel, A.C., Stoeger, H. et al. Antecedents of everyday positive emotions: An experience sampling analysis. Motiv Emot 34, 49–62 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-009-9152-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-009-9152-2

Keywords

Navigation