Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Provider and recipient factors that may moderate the effectiveness of received support: examining the effects of relationship quality and expectations for support on behavioral and cardiovascular reactions

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social relationships have been shown to predict decreased risk for morbidity and mortality. However, the more precise processes underlying these associations are in need of exploration. In this study, we examined important provider (relationship quality) and recipient-related (support expectations) factors that might influence the effectiveness of receiving support on cardiovascular reactivity (CVR). Participants discussed a stressful event with either a supportive or ambivalent friend, and were either given instructions that included an explicit expectation of support provision or no expectation during the task. Behavioral coding revealed fewer emotionally supportive behaviors and marginally more negative behaviors from ambivalent friends. Receiving support from an ambivalent friend was also associated with higher systolic blood pressure reactivity compared to a supportive friend, but had no effects on diastolic blood pressure or heart rate; nor were there any significant statistical interactions with the expectation manipulation. Overall, support expectations had little influence on cardiovascular and behavioral responses. Implications for the study of received support and health are discussed along with potential mechanisms responsible for such links.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There were 16 participants originally classified as supportive that increased in negativity at time 2 such that they no longer met initial classification criteria. Similarly, there were 15 participants originally classified as ambivalent that decreased in negativity such that they no longer met initial criteria at time 2. Subsequent analyses were performed dropping these participants and all major findings remained consistent. Analysis reported in this paper used the original SRI classification (i.e., random assignment).

  2. Results were comparable when we ran the same analyses using gender and baseline cardiovascular measures as covariates.

  3. We did not have any a priori predictions regarding expectation main effects on the behavioral coding. Nevertheless, it should be noted that exploratory analyses did not reveal any such influences on the behavioral categories.

  4. Preliminary analyses examining the participant’s behavior in terms of positive, negative, and off-task behaviors did not reveal any differences as a function of the support expectation manipulation. However, the general nature of these codings do not provide a strong test of behavioral confirmation.

References

  • Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1991). Presence of human friends and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic responses to stress in women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 843–857.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., & Amaral, D. (2007). Effects of support visibility on adjustment to stress: Experimental evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 458–475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N. J., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Invisible support and adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 953–961.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury, T. N., & Pasch, L. A. (1997). Assessment of social support in marital interaction: The social support interaction coding system. Archives of Psychiatric and Clinical Evaluation, 8(2), 147–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Malarkey, W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Uchino, B. N., Sgoutas-Emch, S. A., Sheridan, J. F., et al. (1995). Heterogeneity in neuroendocrine and immune responses to brief psychological stressors as a function of autonomic cardiac activation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 154–164.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campo, R. A., Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Vaughn, A. A., Reblin, M., & Smith, T. W. (2009). The assessment of positivity and negativity in social networks: The reliability and validity of the social relationships index. Journal of Community Psychology, 37, 471–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S. (1988). Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology, 7(3), 269–297.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbet-Owen, C. (2003). Women’s perceptions of partner support in the context of pregnancy loss(es). South African Journal of Psychology, 33(1), 19–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunkel-Schetter, C., & Skokan, L. A. (1990). Determinants of social support provision in personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 437–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finch, J. F., Okun, M. A., & Barrera, M. (1989). Positive and negative social ties among older adults: Measurement models and the prediction of psychological distress and well-being. American Journal of Community Psychology, 17(5), 585–605.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiore, J., Becker, J., & Coppel, D. B. (1983). Social network interactions: A buffer or a stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11(4), 423–439.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., Goff, B. A., & Nadler, A. (1988). Social psychological influences on help seeking and support from peers. In Gottlieb, Benjamin H. (Ed.), Marshaling social support: Formats, processes, and effects (pp. 267–304). Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.

  • Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hansson, R. O., Jones, W. H., & Fletcher, W. L. (1990). Troubled relationships in later life: Implications for support. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 451–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt-Lunstad, J., Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., & Hicks, A. (2007). On the importance of relationship quality: The impact of ambivalence in friendships on cardiovascular functioning. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33(3), 278–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kamarck, T. (1992). Recent developments in the study of cardiovascular reactivity: Contributions from psychometric theory and social psychology. Psychophysiology, 29(5), 491–503.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. In E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. Kelley, R. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the cause of behavior (pp. 1–26). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

  • Kiesler, D. J. (1996). Contemporary interpersonal theory and research: Personality, psychopathology, and psychotherapy. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesler, D. J., Anchin, J. C., Perkins, M. J., Cirico, B. M., Kyle, E. M., & Federman, E. J. (1985). The impact message inventory: Form II. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesler, D. J., Schmidt, J. A., & Wagner, C. C. (1997). A circumplex inventory of impact messages: An operational bridge between emotion and interpersonal behavior. In R. Plutchik & H. R. Conte (Eds.), Circumplex models of personality and emotions (pp. 221–244). Washington, DC: APA.

  • Lakey, B., & Cassady, P. B. (1990). Cognitive processes in perceived social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 337–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakey, B., Moineau, S., & Drew, J. B. (1992). Perceived social support and individual differences in the interpretation and recall of supportive behaviors. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11(4), 336–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadler, A., & Fisher, J. D. (1986). The role of threat to self-esteem and perceived control in recipient reaction to help: Theory development and empirical validation. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 81–122). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nealy, J. B., Smith, T. W., & Uchino, B. N. (2002). Cardiovascular responses to agency and communion stressors in young women. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 395–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2005). Gender differences in social support: A question of skill or responsiveness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 79–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagal, M. D., Erdly, W. W., & Becker, J. (1987). Social networks: We get by with (and in spite of) a little help from our friends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 793–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasch, L. A., Bradbury, T. N., & Davila, J. (1997). Gender, negative affectivity, and observed social support behavior in marital interaction. Personal Relationships, 4, 361–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, T., & Lydon, J. (1998). Priming relational schemas: effects of contextually activated and chronically accessible interpersonal expectations on responses to a stressful event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11(6), 1441–1448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L. T., Lutz, C. J., & Lakey, B. (1999). Perceived social support and attributions for failed support. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(7), 896–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, B. R., Sarason, I. G., Hacker, A., & Basham, R. B. (1985). Concomitants of social support: social skills, physical attractiveness, and gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(2), 469–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 99(2), 143–165.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheffield, D., & Carroll, D. (1994). Social support and cardiovascular reactions to active laboratory stressors. Psychology & Health, 9(4), 305–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, R. C., Wortman, C. B., & Crofton, C. (1990). The role of coping in support provision: The self-presentational dilemma of victims of life crises. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional view (pp. 397–426). Oxford, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W., & MacKenzie, J. (2006). Personality and risk of physical illness. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 435–467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorsteinsson, E. B., & James, J. E. (1999). A meta-analysis of the effects of experimental manipulations of social support during laboratory stress. Psychology and Health, 14, 869–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiber, F., Kamarck, T., Schneiderman, N., Sheffield, D., Kapuku, G., & Taylor, T. (2003). Cardiovascular reactivity and development of preclinical and clinical disease states. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(1), 46–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trobst, K. K. (2000). An interpersonal conceptualization and quantification of social support transactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 971–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uchino, B. N. (2004). Social support and physical health: Understanding the health consequences of our relationships. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uchino, B. N. (2009). Understanding the links between social support and physical health: A lifespan perspective with emphasis on the separability of perceived and received support. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 4, 236–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Uno, D., Campo, R., & Reblin, M. (2007). The social neuroscience of relationships: An examination of health relevant pathways. In E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 474–492). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Uno, D., & Flinders, J. B. (2001). Heterogeneity in the social networks of young and older adults: Prediction of mental health and cardiovascular reactivity during acute stress. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 24, 361–382.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wills, T. A., & Shinar, O. (2000). Measuring perceived and received social support. In S. Cohen, L. Gordon, & B. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support measurement and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 86–135). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maija Reblin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Reblin, M., Uchino, B.N. & Smith, T.W. Provider and recipient factors that may moderate the effectiveness of received support: examining the effects of relationship quality and expectations for support on behavioral and cardiovascular reactions. J Behav Med 33, 423–431 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-010-9270-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-010-9270-z

Keywords

Navigation