Skip to main content
Log in

Uh, Um, and Autism: Filler Disfluencies as Pragmatic Markers in Adolescents with Optimal Outcomes from Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Filler disfluencies—uh and um—are thought to serve distinct discourse functions. We examined fillers in spontaneous speech by youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), who struggle with pragmatic language, and by youth with ASD who have achieved an ‘optimal outcome’ (OO), as well as in peers with typical development (TD). While uh rates did not differ, participants with ASD produced um less frequently than OO or TD groups. Um rate was associated with autism symptom severity, but not executive function or language abilities, suggesting that um serves a pragmatic, listener-oriented function. Moreover, in contrast to minimal production in ASD, the typical OO um production substantiates the normalization of subtle social communication in this population.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders IV-TR (IV-TR Ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

  • Arnold, J. E., Fagnano, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2003). Disfluencies signal theee, um, new information. Journal Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 25–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. E., Hudson Kam, C. L., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007). If you say thee uh you are describing something hard: The on-line attribution of disfluency during reference comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(5), 914–930. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., Altmann, R. J., & Fagnano, M. (2004). The old and thee, uh, new: Disfluency and reference resolution. Psychological Science, 15(9), 578–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., & Seyfeddinipur, M. (2010). The role of fillers in listener attributions for speaker disfluency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(4), 441–455. doi:10.1080/01690960903047122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bortfeld, H., Leon, S. D., Bloom, J. E., Schober, M. F., & Brennan, S. E. (2001). Disfluency rates in conversation: Effects of age, relationship, topic, role, and gender. Language and Speech, 44(2), 123–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, S. E., & Williams, M. (1995). The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 383–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Fox Tree, J. (2002). Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition, 84(1), 73–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & van der Lely, H. K. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 28–40. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0357-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis–Kaplan executive function system (D–KEFS) examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelhardt, P. E., Ferreira, F., & Nigg, J. T. (2011). Language production strategies and disfluencies in multi-clause network descriptions: A study of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 25(4), 442–453. doi:10.1037/a0022436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engelhardt, P. E., Nigg, J. T., & Ferreira, F. (2013). Is the fluency of language outputs related to individual differences in intelligence and executive function? Acta Psychologica, 144(2), 424–432. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.08.002.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fein, D., Barton, M., Eigsti, I. M., Kelley, E., Naigles, L., Schultz, R. T., & Tyson, K. (2013). Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2), 195–205.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, A., Fein, D. A., & Eigsti, I. M. (2015). Detail and gestalt focus in individuals with optimal outcomes from autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(6), 1887–1896. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2347-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H., & Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica: Journal of the International Association for Semiotic Studies/Revue de l’ Association Internationale de Sémiotique, 62(1–2), 51–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, Z., & Spieler, D. (2006). Observing the what and when of language production for different age groups by monitoring speakers’ eye movements. Brain and Language, 99(3), 272–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helt, M., Kelley, E., Kinsbourne, M., Pandey, J., Boorstein, H., Herbert, M., & Fein, D. (2008). Can children with autism recover? If so, how? Neuropsychology Reviews, 18(4), 339–366. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9075-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 151–171. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, E., Naigles, L., & Fein, D. (2010). An in-depth examination of optimal outcome children with a history of autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 526–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, E., Paul, J. J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(6), 807–828.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, C., White, K. S., & Aslin, R. N. (2011). Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers’ referential intentions. Developmental Science, 14(4), 925–934.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, C., Gorman, K., Yuan, J., & Liberman, M. (2007). Perception of disfluency: Language differences and listener bias. Paper presented at the annual conference of the international speech communication association, Antwerp, Belgium.

  • Lake, J. K., Humphreys, K. R., & Cardy, S. (2011). Listener vs. speaker-oriented aspects of speech: Studying the disfluencies of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18(1), 135–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maclay, H., & Osgood, C. E. (1959). Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech. Word-Journal of the International Linguistic Association, 15(1), 19–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesibov, G. (1992). Treatment issues with high-functioning adolescents and adults with autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 143–156). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Oviatt, S. L. (1995). Predicting spoken disfluencies during human–computer interaction. Computer Speech and Language, 9(1), 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2009). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 115–125.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Shriberg, L., McSweeney, J., Cicchetti, D., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Relations between prosodic performance and communication and socialization ratings in high functioning speakers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 861–869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plexico, L. W., Cleary, J. E., McAlpine, A., & Plumb, A. M. (2010). Disfluency characteristics observed in young children with autism spectrum disorders: A preliminary report. SIG 4 Perspectives on Fluency and Fluency Disorders, 20(2), 42–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., Christenfeld, N., & Ravina, B. (1991). Speech disfluency and the structure of knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(3), 362–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, E. E. (1994). Preliminaries to a theory of speech disfluencies. Oakland: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., Black, L. M., & van Santen, J. P. (2011). The hypothesis of apraxia of speech in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(4), 405–426.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A. M., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(5), 1097–1115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V. L., & Clark, H. H. (1993). On the course of answering questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suh, J., Eigsti, I. M., Naigles, L., Barton, M., Kelley, E., & Fein, D. (2014). Narrative performance of optimal outcome children and adolescents with a history of an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 6, 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (1st ed.). New York: Pearson Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from National Institutes of Mental Health (R01 MH076189) to D.F. We would like to thank Alyssa Orenstein, Eva Troyb, Katherine Tyson, Michael Rosenthal, and Molly Helt for their assistance with data collection, Allison Fitch, Sydney Seese, and Erica Crowley for their assistance with transcription, and the parents and children who participated in this study.

Author Contributions

CI conceived of the study, participated in its design, performed analyses, and drafted the manuscript; IME participated in the design, analyses, and interpretation of the data, and helped to draft the manuscript; DAF participated in the design and coordination of the study and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inge-Marie Eigsti.

Additional information

The present study was conducted by Christina A. Irvine, M.A., University of Connecticut, Storrs, Inge-Marie Eigsti, Ph.D., University of Connecticut, and Deborah Fein, Ph.D., University of Connecticut.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Irvine, C.A., Eigsti, IM. & Fein, D.A. Uh, Um, and Autism: Filler Disfluencies as Pragmatic Markers in Adolescents with Optimal Outcomes from Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 1061–1070 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2651-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2651-y

Keywords

Navigation