Skip to main content
Log in

Multiple Object Tracking in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Difficulties in visual attention are often implicated in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) but it remains unclear which aspects of attention are affected. Here, we used a multiple object tracking (MOT) task to quantitatively characterize dynamic attentional function in children with ASD aged 5–12. While the ASD group performed significantly worse overall, the group difference did not increase with increased object speed. This finding suggests that decreased MOT performance is not due to deficits in dynamic attention but instead to a diminished capacity to select and maintain attention on multiple targets. Further, MOT performance improved from 5 to 10 years in both typical and ASD groups with similar developmental trajectories. These results argue against a specific deficit in dynamic attention in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Attention function and dysfunction in autism. Frontiers in Bioscience: A Journal and Virtual Library, 6, D105–D119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D. G., & Milne, A. (2006). Multiple-target tracking: A role for working memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 1101–1116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Althaus, M., deSonneville, L. M. J., Minderaa, R. B., Hensen, L. G. N., & Til, R. B. (1996). Information processing and aspects of visual attention in children with the DSM-III-R diagnosis ‘‘pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified’’ (PDDNOS).1. Focused and divided attention. Child Neuropsychology, 2(1), 17–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American, P. P. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

  • Amirault, M., Etchegoyhen, K., Delord, S., Mendizabal, S., Kraushaar, C., Hesling, I., et al. (2009). Alteration of attentional blink in high functioning autism: A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(11), 1522–1528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Annaz, D., Remington, A., Milne, E., Coleman, M., Campbell, R., Thomas, M. S., et al. (2010). Development of motion processing in children with autism. Developmental Science, 13(6), 826–838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashida, H., Seiffert, A. E., & Osaka, N. (2001). Inefficient visual search for second-order motion. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 18(9), 2255–2266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47(13), 3023–3029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldassi, S., Pei, F., Megna, N., Recupero, G., Viespoli, M., Igliozzi, R., et al. (2009). Search superiority in autism within, but not outside the crowding regime. Vision Research, 49(16), 2151–2156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beaton, E. A., Stoddard, J., Lai, S., Lackey, J., Shi, J., Ross, J. L., et al. (2010). Atypical functional brain activation during a multiple object tracking task in girls with Turner syndrome: Neurocorrelates of reduced spatiotemporal resolution. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 115(2), 140–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67(4), 1816–1835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128(Pt 10), 2430–2441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bettencourt, K. C., & Somers, D. C. (2009). Effects of target enhancement and distractor suppression on multiple object tracking capacity. Journal of Vision, 9(7), 9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14(2), 151–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bogte, H., Flamma, B., Van Der Meere, J., & Van Engeland, H. (2009). Divided attention capacity in adults with autism spectrum disorders and without intellectual disability. Autism, 13(3), 229–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brieber, S., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Fink, G. R., Kamp-Becker, I., Remschmidt, H., & Konrad, K. (2010). Coherent motion processing in autism spectrum disorder (ASD): An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1644–1651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Brown, C. C., Boucher, J., & Rippon, G. (2002). The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 209–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bulakowski, P. F., Bressler, D. W., & Whitney, D. (2007). Shared attentional resources for global and local motion processing. Journal of Vision, 7(10), 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(3), 535–543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bussing, R., Fernandez, M., Harwood, M., Hou, W., Garvan, C. W., Eyberg, S. M., et al. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sample. Assessment, 15(3), 317–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanagh, P., & Alvarez, G. A. (2005). Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 349–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavanagh, P., Labianca, A. T., & Thornton, I. M. (2001). Attention-based visual routines: Sprites. Cognition, 80(1–2), 47–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ciesielski, K. T., Courchesne, E., & Elmasian, R. (1990). Effects of focused selective attention tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and normal individuals. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75(3), 207–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ciesielski, K. T., Knight, J. E., Prince, R. J., Harris, R. J., & Handmaker, S. D. (1995). Event-related potentials in cross-modal divided attention in autism. Neuropsychologia, 33(2), 225–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, J., Saygin, A. P., Swain, R., & Blakemore, S. J. (2009). Reduced sensitivity to minimum-jerk biological motion in autism spectrum conditions. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3275–3278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Culham, J. C., Brandt, S. A., Cavanagh, P., Kanwisher, N. G., Dale, A. M., & Tootell, R. B. (1998). Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(5), 2657–2670.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Culham, J. C., Cavanagh, P., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Attention response functions: Characterizing brain areas using fMRI activation during parametric variations of attentional load. Neuron, 32(4), 737–745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 729–745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Holmboe, K., Tucker, L., Csibra, G., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2009). Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: Disengagement and facilitation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 50(5), 637–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Facoetti, A., Ruffino, M., Peru, A., Paganoni, P., & Chelazzi, L. (2008). Sluggish engagement and disengagement of non-spatial attention in dyslexic children. Cortex, 44(9), 1221–1233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foss-Feig, J. H., Kwakye, L. D., Cascio, C. J., Burnette, C. P., Kadivar, H., Stone, W. L., et al. (2010). An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 203(2), 381–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franconeri, S. L., Jonathan, S. V., & Scimeca, J. M. (2010). Tracking multiple objects is limited only by object spacing, not by speed, time, or capacity. Psychological Science, 21(7), 920–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franconeri, S. L., Lin, J. Y., Pylyshyn, Z. W., Fisher, B., & Enns, J. T. (2008). Evidence against a speed limit in multiple-object tracking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 802–808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Häberlen, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., et al. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1480–1494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70(4), 817–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Groen, W. B., van Orsouw, L., Huurne, N., Swinkels, S., van der Gaag, R. J., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2009). Intact spectral but abnormal temporal processing of auditory stimuli in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(5), 742–750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, P. D., Horowitz, T. S., Morocz, I. A., Wolfe, J., & Livingstone, M. S. (2009). Using fMRI to distinguish components of the multiple object tracking task. Journal of Vision, 9(4), 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hulleman, J. (2005). The mathematics of multiple object tracking: From proportions correct to number of objects tracked. Vision Research, 45(17), 2298–2309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibanez, L. V., Messinger, D. S., Newell, L., Lambert, B., & Sheskin, M. (2008). Visual disengagement in the infant siblings of children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Autism, 12(5), 473–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 4(5), 347–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2009). Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Developmental Science, 12(6), 1083–1096.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jovicich, J., Peters, R. J., Koch, C., Braun, J., Chang, L., & Ernst, T. (2001). Brain areas specific for attentional load in a motion-tracking task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(8), 1048–1058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, M. D., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A review and synthesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 761–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Toddlers with autism spectrum disorder are more successful at visual search than typically developing toddlers. Developmental Science, 14(5), 980–988.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS, Pearson, Inc.

  • Kikuchi, Y., Senju, A., Akechi, H., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 629–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. M. (2010). The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. Brain, 133(Pt2), 599–610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. M. (2011). Neural correlates of coherent and biological motion perception in autism. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1075–1088.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Cascio, C. J., Stone, W. L., & Wallace, M. T. (2011). Altered auditory and multisensory temporal processing in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4, 129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Hoffman, J. E., Reiss, J. E., Dilks, D. D., Lakusta, L., & Chunyo, G. (2006). Specialization, breakdown, and sparing in spatial cognition: Lessons from Williams syndrome. In C. Morris, H. Lenhoff, & P. Wang (Eds.), Williams-Beuren syndrome: Research and clinical perspectives. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

  • Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 1115–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lepage, J. F., Dunkin, B., Hong, D. S., & Reiss, A. L. (2011). Contribution of executive functions to visuospatial difficulties in prepubertal girls with Turner syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(8), 988–1002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of Autistic Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(4), 445–460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. J., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61(4), 474–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, T. A., & Walker, P. (2003). Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(2), 274–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzocco, M. M., Singh Bhatia, N., & Lesniak-Karpiak, K. (2006). Visuospatial skills and their association with math performance in girls with fragile X or Turner syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 12(2), 87–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(2), 255–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., & Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 225–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullane, J. C., & Klein, R. M. (2008). Literature review: Visual search by children with and without ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12(1), 44–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., & Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3225–3235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakano, T., Ota, H., Kato, N., & Kitazawa, S. (2010). Deficit in visual temporal integration in autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 277(1684), 1027–1030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newsome, W. T., & Pare, E. B. (1988). A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). Journal of Neuroscience, 8(6), 2201–2211.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hearn, K., Hoffman, J. E., & Landau, B. (2010). Developmental profiles for multiple object tracking and spatial memory: Typically developing preschoolers and people with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 13(3), 430–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hearn, K., Landau, B., & Hoffman, J. E. (2005). Multiple object tracking in people with Williams syndrome and in normally developing children. Psychological Science, 16(11), 905–912.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism, 8(3), 229–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(4), 961–979.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 257–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., & Gibson, L. Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 46(10), 2593–2596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43(7), 1044–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Shultz, S., Hudac, C. M., & Vander Wyk, B. C. (2011). Research review: Constraining heterogeneity: The social brain and its development in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52(6), 631–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, K., Glad, K. S., & Schreibman, L. (1997). Social perception in children with autism: An attentional deficit? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(3), 265–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 777–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 733–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poirier, M., Martin, J. S., Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2011). Short-term memory in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(1), 247–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3(3), 179–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Swettenham, J., Campbell, R., & Coleman, M. (2009). Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1388–1393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 5(1), 67–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N., Tonge, B., Brereton, A., & Bradshaw, J. (2010). Attentional blink in young people with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 14(1), 47–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Henry, L. (1996). Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37(6), 673–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 77–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., Richards, E. D., Moldes, V., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Evidence of a divided-attention advantage in autism. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(5), 505–515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., & Troje, N. F. (2012). IQ predicts biological motion perception in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 557–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). SCQ: Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

  • Saygin, A. P., Cook, J., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE, 5(10), e13491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shim, W. M., Alvarez, G. A., & Jiang, Y. V. (2008). Spatial separation between targets constrains maintenance of attention on multiple objects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 390–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shim, W. M., Alvarez, G. A., Vickery, T. J., & Jiang, Y. V. (2010). The number of attentional foci and their precision are dissociated in the posterior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 20(6), 1341–1349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J. V., & O’Brien, J. M. (2006). Visual form-processing deficits in autism. Perception, 35(8), 1047–1055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. NeuroReport, 11(12), 2765–2767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, I. M., Rensink, R. A., & Shiffrar, M. (2002). Active versus passive processing of biological motion. Perception, 31(7), 837–853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tombu, M., & Seiffert, A. E. (2008). Attentional costs in multiple-object tracking. Cognition, 108(1), 1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J., Harris, N. S., & Courchesne, E. (1996). Visual attention abnormalities in autism: Delayed orienting to location. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(6), 541–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., Jaspers-Fayer, F., & Sethi, N. (2005). Multiple-object tracking in children: The catch the spies task. Cognitive Development, 20(3), 373–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., Mutreja, R., & Hunt, K. (2012). Spatial and visuospatial working memory tests predict performance in classic multiple-object tracking in young adults, but nonspatial measures of the executive do not. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 74(2), 300–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsermentseli, S., O’Brien, J. M., & Spencer, J. V. (2008). Comparison of form and motion coherence processing in autistic spectrum disorders and dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1201–1210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Camfferman, G., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2001). Eye movements, visual attention, and autism: A saccadic reaction time study using the gap and overlap paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 50(8), 614–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenbroucke, M. W., Scholte, H. S., van Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A., & Kemner, C. (2008). Coherent versus component motion perception in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 941–949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception & Psychophysics, 33(2), 113–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would especially like to thank all the participants and their families for their time and contribution to our research. We are grateful to all of the families at the participating SFARI Simplex Collection (SSC) sites, as well as the principal investigators (A. Beaudet, R. Bernier, J. Constantino, E. Cook, E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, E. Hanson, D. Grice, A. Klin, R. Kochel, D. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. Martin, D. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, K. Pelphrey, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. State, W. Stone, J. Sutcliffe, C. Walsh, Z. Warren, E. Wijsman). We are also grateful to all of the families participating in the Autism Consortium collection, as well as the principal investigators. This study was supported by funds from the Ellison Medical Foundation and by a grant from the Simons Foundation to the Simons Center for the Social Brain at MIT.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kami Koldewyn.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koldewyn, K., Weigelt, S., Kanwisher, N. et al. Multiple Object Tracking in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 43, 1394–1405 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1694-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1694-6

Keywords

Navigation