Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Variation in Parasympathetic Dysregulation Moderates Short-term Memory Problems in Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 03 March 2018

An Erratum to this article was published on 26 September 2015

This article has been updated

Abstract

Although attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with impairment in working memory and short-term memory, up to half of individual children with ADHD perform within a normative range. Heterogeneity in other ADHD-related mechanisms, which may compensate for or combine with cognitive weaknesses, is a likely explanation. One candidate is the robustness of parasympathetic regulation (as indexed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia; RSA). Theory and data suggest that a common neural network is likely tied to both heart-rate regulation and certain cognitive functions (including aspects of working and short-term memory). Cardiac-derived indices of parasympathetic reactivity were collected during short-term memory (STM) storage and rehearsal tasks from 243 children (116 ADHD, 127 controls). ADHD was associated with lower STM performance, replicating previous work. In addition, RSA reactivity moderated the association between STM and ADHD – both as a category and a dimension – independent of comorbidity. Specifically, conditional effects revealed that high levels of withdrawal interacted with weakened STM but high levels of augmentation moderated a positive association predicting ADHD. Thus, variations in parasympathetic reactivity may help explain neuropsychological heterogeneity in ADHD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 03 March 2018

    The authors note two errors in this article. In the reference section, Musser et al. (2014) was mistakenly used whereas the following entry should have been used.

  • 26 September 2015

    An erratum to this article has been published.

Notes

  1. Extant work has suggested that many commonly used simple span tasks such as those used here consistently load on a factor typically referred to as short-term memory (i.e., storage or rehearsal abilities) while more complex tasks better index working memory (see Engle 2002). However, as evidenced in meta-analytic literature (Martinussen et al. 2005; Willcutt et al. 2005), these simple span tasks are often employed as measures of working memory. Thus, to increase the generalizability of findings to previous studies, we utilize simple span tasks but have chosen to use the terms short-term memory storage and rehearsal to reflect this advancement.

  2. Moderation analyses were also conducted using ADHD Inattentive and Combined presentations as categorical outcomes. Findings largely mirrored those presented here and below. Specific findings are available upon request.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060–1073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten, A. F., & Li, B. M. (2005). Neurobiology of executive functions: catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1377–1384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought and action. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2005). Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 40, 373–400. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr4003_5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and gray’s motivational theory: toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchaine, T. P., Katkin, E. S., Strassberg, Z., & Snarr, J. (2001). Disinhibitory psychopathology in male adolescents: discriminating conduct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder through concurrent assessment of multiple autonomic states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 610–624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007). Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology: emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74, 174–184. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F., … & Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 757.

  • Bunge, S. A., & Wright, S. B. (2007). Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 243–250. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calkins, S. D., Graziano, P. A., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biological Psychology, 74, 144–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, J., Engle, R. W., & Hamilton, G. (1991). Short-term memory, working memory, and verbal abilities: how do they relate? Intelligence, 15, 229–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conradt, E., Degarmo, D., Fisher, P., Abar, B., Lester, B. M., Lagasse, L. L., … & Hammond, J. A. (2014). The contributions of early adverse experiences and trajectories of respiratory sinus arrhythmia on the development of neurobehavioral disinhibition among children with prenatal substance exposure. Development and psychopathology, 26, 901–916.

  • Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Sylvers, P., Mead, H., & Chipman-Chacon, J. (2006). Autonomic correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD rating scale—IV: Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fray, P. J., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (1996). Neuropsychiatric applications of CANTAB. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 329–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Froehlich, T. E., Lanphear, B. P., Epstein, J. N., Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., & Kahn, R. S. (2007). Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of U.S. children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 161, 857–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gianaros, P. J., Van der Veen, F. M., & Jennings, J. R. (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow correlates with heart period and high-frequency heart period variability during working-memory tasks: implications for the cortical and subcortical regulation of cardiac autonomic activity. Psychophysiology, 41, 521–530. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00179.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, P., & Derefinko, K. (2013). Cardiac vagal control and children’s adaptive functioning: a meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 94, 22–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gropper, R. J., & Tannock, R. (2009). A pilot study of working memory and academic achievement in college students with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 574–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh, F. Y. (1989). Sample size tables for logistic regression. Statistics in Medicine, 8, 795–802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R., & Figueroa, R. A. (1975). Forward and backward digit span interaction with race and IQ: predictions from Jensen’s theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karalunas, S. L., Fair, D., Musser, E. D., Aykes, K., Iyer, S. P., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Subtyping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using temperament dimensions: toward biologically based nosologic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 1015–1024. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.763.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lambek, R., Tannock, R., Dalsgaard, S., Trillingsgaard, A., Damm, D., & Thomsen, P. H. (2011). Executive dysfunction in school-age children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15, 646–655. doi:10.1177/1087054710370935.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loeber, R., Green, S., Lahey, B., Frick, P., & McBurnett, K. (2000). Findings on disruptive behavior disorders from the first decade of the developmental trends study. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3, 37–60. doi:10.1023/A:1009567419190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcovitch, S., Leigh, J., Calkins, S. D., Leerks, E. M., O’Brien, M., & Blankson, A. N. (2010). Moderate vagal withdrawal in 3.5‐year‐old children is associated with optimal performance on executive function tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 603–608. doi:10.1002/dev.20462.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MindWare. (2008). MindWare heart rate variability V. 2.6 system. Gahanna: MindWare Technologies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musser, E. D., Backs, R. W., Schmitt, C. F., Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., & Nigg, J. T. (2011). Emotion regulation via the autonomic nervous system in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 841–852.

  • Musser, E. D., Hawkey, E., Kachan‐Liu, S. S., Lees, P., Roullet, J. B., Goddard, K., ... & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Shared familial transmission of autism spectrum and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 819–827.

  • Negrao, B. L., Bipath, P., van der Westhuizen, D., & Viljoen, M. (2011). Autonomic correlates at rest and during evoked attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and effects of methylphenidate. Neuropsychobiology, 63(2), 82–91. doi:10.1159/00031754.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2012). Future directions in ADHD etiology research. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 41, 524–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T., & Casey, B. J. (2005). An integrative theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 785–806. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050376.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1224–1230. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2001). The Polyvagal Theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 123–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74, 116–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raiker, J. S., Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., & Sarver, D. E. (2012). Objectively-measured impulsivity and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): testing competing predictions from the working memory and behavioral inhibition models of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 699–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Chung, K. M., Shore, G., & Isaacs, P. (2001). A conceptual model of child psychopathology: implications for understanding attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and treatment efficacy. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 48–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Alderson, R. M., Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Bolden, J., & Sims, V. (2008). Working memory deficits in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the contribution of central executive and subsystem processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 825–837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rash, J. A., & Aguirre-Camacho, A. (2012). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and cardiac vagal control: a systematic review. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 4, 167–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1231–1238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Dalen, L., & Remington, B. (2003). Do executive deficits and delay aversion make independent contributions to preschool attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder symptoms? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soper, D.S. (2015). Post-hoc statistical power calculator for multiple regression [Software]. Available from http://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc.

  • Suess, P. E., Porges, S. W., & Plude, D. J. (1994). Cardiac vagal tone and sustained attention in school-age children. Psychophysiology, 31, 17–22. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01020.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., & Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 141–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wager, T. D., & Smith, E. E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 255–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., … & Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 991.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01-MH59105 awarded to Joel T. Nigg, Ph. D.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anthony R. Ward.

Additional information

A correction to this article is available online at https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0406-x.

An erratum to this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0083-y.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ward, A.R., Alarcón, G., Nigg, J.T. et al. Variation in Parasympathetic Dysregulation Moderates Short-term Memory Problems in Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 43, 1573–1583 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0054-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0054-3

Keywords

Navigation