Skip to main content
Log in

What Part of Working Memory is not Working in ADHD? Short-Term Memory, the Central Executive and Effects of Reinforcement

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Deficits in Working Memory (WM) are related to symptoms of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). In children with ADHD visuospatial WM is most impaired. WM is composed of Short-Term Memory (STM) and a Central Executive (CE). Therefore, deficits in either or both STM and the CE may account for WM impairments in children with ADHD. WM-component studies investigating this find deficits in both STM and the CE. However, recent studies show that not only cognitive deficits, but also motivational deficits give rise to the aberrant WM performance of children with ADHD. To date, the influence of these motivational deficits on the components of WM has not been investigated. This study examined the effects of a standard (feedback-only) and a high level of reinforcement (feedback + 10 euros) on the visuospatial WM-, visuospatial STM-, and the CE performance of 86 children with ADHD and 62 typically-developing controls. With standard reinforcement the STM, CE, and WM performance of children with ADHD was worse than that of controls. High reinforcement improved STM and WM performance more in children with ADHD than in controls, but was unable to normalize their performance. High reinforcement did not appear to improve the CE-related performance of children with ADHD and controls. Motivational deficits have a detrimental effect on both the visuospatial WM performance and the STM performance of children with ADHD. Aside from motivational deficits, both the visuospatial STM and the CE of children with ADHD are impaired, and give rise to their deficits in visuospatial WM.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Operationalizing central executive performance by using the difference between working memory performance and short-term memory performance is based upon the theorem of Engle et al. (1999) — which is consistent with other influential working memory models like those of Cowan (1995) and Baddeley and Hitch (1974) — that the working memory system consists of the contents of short-term memory plus the central executive. According to Engle et al. “working memory capacity = short-term memory capacity + central executive + the error of measurement” (p. 313).

  2. Orders of presentation used in counterbalancing:

    1.

    FO: STM > WG

    >

    10 euros: STM > WG

    2.

    10 euros: STM > WG

    >

    FO: STM > WG

    3.

    FO: WG > STM

    >

    10 euros: WG > STM

    4.

    10 euros: WG > STM

    >

    FO: WG > STM

    5.

    FO: STM > WG

    >

    10 euros: WG > STM

    6.

    10 euros: STM > WG

    >

    FO: WG > STM

    7.

    FO: WG > STM

    >

    10 euros: STM > WG

    8.

    10 euros: WG > STM

    >

    FO: STM > WG

    STM short-term memory; WM working memory; FO Feedback-only

  3. If the 10 euros were given contingent upon performance (for instance after 20 correct trials) then the number of performed trials (i.e. the work load) in the 10 euros condition could differ between participants and groups (e.g., with some children needing 24 trials to reach 20 correct trials and other children needing 34 trials). This difference in work load between participants and groups in the 10 euro condition could then result in differences in task performance in the subsequent reinforcement condition (e.g. due to more fatigue in one group than in the other).

  4. The task started at a very easy level (a sequence of two stimuli), and because the difficulty level of the task adapts gradually (see above), children typically needed the first 12 trials to reach their optimal difficulty level (a sequence length higher than 5 or 6 stimuli). Since the mean of these first 12 trials gave no relevant information on individual performance, and inclusion of these trials resulted in a more inaccurate representation of participant’s short-term memory and working memory capacity, these first trials were excluded from analysis (results did not change when the first 12 trials were included; see also Dovis et al. 2012).

  5. Without covarying for IQ, gender, and weekly spendable income the pattern of the results was the same. Further, IQ, gender, and weekly spendable income did not significantly interact with Reinforcement condition or Task version.

  6. However, note that such a strategy may cause a procedural confound when the impact of reward on performance is compared between groups: If the total amount of reward that can be earned is contingent upon cognitive performance (i.e. when better performance results in a higher reward), then a participant’s cognitive ability is likely to influence the maximum amount of reward this participant can earn. This means that a group characterized by cognitive impairments (e.g., ADHD) is likely to receive less rewards than a typically developing group. However, to properly compare the impact of reward on performance between two such groups, it is required that the amount of reward in both groups is the same. Otherwise, it cannot be determined whether a difference in the impact of reward on performance between these two groups is the result of the difference in symptomatology between these groups or of the difference in the presented amount of reward.

References

  • Aarts, E., van Holstein, M., & Cools, R. (2011). Striatal dopamine and the interface between motivation and cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alderson, R. M., Rapport, M. D., Hudec, K. L., Sarver, D. E., & Kofler, M. J. (2010). Competing core processes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): do working memory deficiencies underlie behavioral inhibition deficits? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th revised edition (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20, R136–R140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S. J., Hanson, C. A., Puffenberger, S. S., Benninger, K. L., & Benninger, W. B. (2010). A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 825–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, G. C., Depue, B. E., Ruzic, L., Willcutt, E. G., Du, Y. P., & Banich, M. T. (2010). Attentional control activation relates to working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 632–640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 117–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Jarrold, C., Kane, M. J., Miyake, A., & Towse, J. (2007). Variation in working memory: An introduction. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cools, R., & D’Esposito, M. (2011). Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 113–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 819B.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delaney, H. D., & Maxwell, S. E. (1981). On using analysis of covariance in repeated measures designs. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 16, 105–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demurie, E., Roeyers, H., Baeyens, D., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2011). Common alterations in sensitivity to type but not amount of reward in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 1164–1173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 15, 331–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2005). Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): a neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity). Development and Psychopathology, 17, 807–825.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R. W., & Prins, P. J. M. (2012). Can motivation normalize working memory and task persistence in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? The effects of money and computer-gaming. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 669–681.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durstewitz, D., & Seamans, J. K. (2002). The computational role of dopamine D1 receptors in working memory. Neural Networks, 15, 561–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durstewitz, D., & Seamans, J. K. (2008). The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 739–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 128, 309–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. J., Moustafa, A. A., Haughey, H. M., Curran, T., & Hutchison, K. E. (2007). Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 16311–16316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. E., Figner, B., Crone, E. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 364–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenhill, L. L. (1998). Childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Pharmacological treatments. In P. E. Nathan & J. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (pp. 42–64). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadwin, J. A., Brogan, J., & Stevenson, J. (2005). State anxiety and working memory in children: a test of processing efficiency theory. Educational Psychology, 25, 379–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haenlein, M., & Caul, W. F. (1987). Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: a specific hypothesis of reward dysfunction. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 356–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang-Pollock, C. L., Mikami, A. Y., Pfiffner, L., & McBurnett, K. (2007). ADHD subtype differences in motivational responsivity but not inhibitory control: evidence from a reward-based variation of the stop signal paradigm. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 127–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kittler, J. E., Menard, W., & Phillips, K. A. (2007). Weight concerns in individuals with body dysmorphic disorder. Eating Behaviors, 8, 115–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Bolden, J., Sarver, D. E., & Raiker, J. S. (2010). ADHD and working memory: the impact of central executive deficits and exceeding storage/rehearsal capacity on observed inattentive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 149–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kort, W., Compaan, E. L., Bleichrodt, N., Resing, W. C. M., Schittekatte, M., Bosmans, M., et al. (2002). WISC-III NL Handleiding. (Dutch Manual). Amsterdam: NIP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 377–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., Chase, G. A., Mink, D. M., & Stagg, R. E. (2009). ADHD subtypes and co-occurring anxiety, depression, and oppositional-defiant disorder: differences in Gordon diagnostic system and Wechsler working memory and processing speed index scores. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 540–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, K., & Hikosaka, O. (2006). Role of dopamine in the primate caudate nucleus in reward modulation of saccades. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26, 5360–5369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2006). What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., Scheres, A., Antrop, I., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2000). Vragenlijst voor Gedragsproblemen bij Kinderen (VvGK) (Dutch translation of the DBD Rating Scale). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatment for ADHD: an update. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37, 184–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Greenslade, K. E., & Milich, R. (1992). Teacher rating of DSM-III-R symptoms for disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 8, 259–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raiker, J. S., Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., & Sarver, D. E. (2012). Objectively-measured impulsivity and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): testing competing predictions from the working memory and behavioral inhibition models of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 699–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Alderson, R. M., Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Bolden, J., & Sims, V. (2008). Working memory deficits in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the contribution of central executive and subsystem processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 825–837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Bolden, J., Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., & Alderson, R. M. (2009). Hyperactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a ubiquitous core symptom or manifestation of working memory deficits? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 521–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, E. J., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2006). Pattern of impaired working memory during major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 90, 149–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarkis, S. M., Sarkis, E. H., Marshall, D., & Archer, J. (2005). Self-regulation and inhibition in comorbid ADHD children: an evaluation of executive functions. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8, 96–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). San Diego: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatz, D. B., & Rostain, A. L. (2006). ADHD with comorbid anxiety: a review of the current literature. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 141–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheres, A., Milham, M. P., Knutson, B., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 720–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheres, A., Lee, A., & Sumiya, M. (2008). Temporal reward discounting and ADHD: task and symptom specific effects. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 221–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., Oosterlaan, J., & Van der Meere, J. (1999). Information processing and energetic factors in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In H. C. Quay & A. E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders (pp. 75–104). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 28–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiels, K., Hawk, L. W., Jr., Lysczek, C. L., Tannock, R., Pelham, W. E., Jr., Spencer, S. V., et al. (2008). The effects of incentives on visual-spatial working memory in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 903–913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Research, 130, 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J. T., & Willcutt, E. G. (2008). Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in ADHD: nosological and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17, 367–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Bitsakou, P., & Thompson, M. (2010). Beyond the dual pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory control, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 345–355.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764–1778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strand, M. T., Hawk, L. W., Bubnik, M., Shiels, K., Pelham, W. E., & Waxmonsky, J. G. (2012). Improving working memory in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the separate and combined effects of incentives and stimulant medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 1193–1207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ströhle, A., Stoy, M., Wrase, J., Schwarzer, S., Schlagenhauf, F., Huss, et al. (2008). Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage, 39, 966–972.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tillman, C., Eninger, L., Forssman, L., & Bohlin, G. (2011). The relation between working memory components and ADHD symptoms from a developmental perspective. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 181–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tripp, G., & Wickens, J. R. (2008). Research review: dopamine transfer deficit: a neurobiological theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 49, 691–704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Meere, J., Hughes, K. A., Börger, N., & Sallee, F. R. (1995). The effect of reward on sustained attention in AD/HD children with and without CD. In J. A. Sergeant (Ed.), European approach to hyperkinetic disorder (pp. 241–253). Zurich: Fotorotar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Oord, S., Ponsioen, A. J. G. B., Geurts, H. M., Ten Brink, E. L., & Prins, P. J. M. (2012). A pilot study of the efficacy of a computerized executive functioning remediation training with game elements for children with ADHD in an outpatient setting: outcome on parent- and teacher-rated executive functioning and ADHD behavior. Journal of Attention Disorders. doi:10.1177/1087054712453167.

  • Walsh, N. D., Williams, S. C. R., Brammer, M. J., Bullmore, E. T., Kim, J., Suckling, J., et al. (2007). A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of verbal working memory in depression after antidepressant therapy. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 1236–1243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence (4th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Witkins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM–IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 991–1010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Jeugdriagg Noord Holland Zuid, GGz Noord Holland Noord (Centrum voor Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie), Regionaal Centrum voor Kinder en Jeugdpsychiatrie Gooi en Vechtstreek (RCKJP), Bosman GGz, Stichting De Praktijk, Stichting Kram, PuntP, Academisch Behandelcentrum UvA Minds, Kinderpraktijk VIS and the participating schools (OBS De Weidevogel, Amsterdam; OBS De Witte Olifant, Amsterdam; De Dr. E. Boekmanschool, Amsterdam; OBS Jules Verne, Alkmaar; PCBS Van der Brugghenschool, Huizen; Montessorischool De Boog, Nieuw-Vennep; and De Willemsparkschool, Amsterdam), to Thomas Gladwin for his comments and statistical advice, to Jasper Wijnen for programming the task, to Tim van den Broek, Josje de Bont, Annette Brouwer, Tycho Dekkers, Lucie van den Eertwegh, Roza van der Heide, Lisanne Klink, Astrid Nauta, Inge Meulenberg, Muriël Musa, Pascale Riaskoff, Elise Tilma, Marije Voermans, Ida de Vries, and Pamina Warmbrunn for their help with data collection, and to all participating children and families.

Disclosures

SvdO has been a paid consultant for Janssen pharmaceuticals in the development and evaluation of a serious game “Heelseeker” aimed at training cognitive functions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sebastiaan Dovis.

Appendix

Appendix

1. The reinforcement instruction for the first presented task version in the Feedback-Only condition (translated from Dutch):

figure a

2. The reinforcement instruction for the second presented task version in the Feedback-Only condition (translated from Dutch):

figure b

3. The reinforcement instruction for the first presented task version in the 10 euros condition (translated from Dutch):

figure c

4. The reinforcement instruction for the second presented task version in the 10 euros condition (translated from Dutch):

figure d

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R.W. et al. What Part of Working Memory is not Working in ADHD? Short-Term Memory, the Central Executive and Effects of Reinforcement. J Abnorm Child Psychol 41, 901–917 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9729-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9729-9

Keywords

Navigation