Skip to main content
Log in

Sexual Selection in the Loud Calls of Male Primates: Signal Content and Function

  • Published:
International Journal of Primatology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Researchers have used sexual selection theory and hypotheses based on intersexual mate choice and intrasexual mate competition to explain the role of spontaneous long-distance vocalizations emitted by adult male primates, relying on the tacit assumption that assessment or identity cues are encoded in the vocalizations. I review the published literature and aim to substantiate a relationship between sexual selection and long-distance vocal communication in primates. First, I review findings from nonprimate taxa to determine the relative importance of inter- and intrasexual selection and to provide a background for examining primates. Next, I describe several hypotheses for signal content and function in adult male loud calls. Then, I examine the available data across Primates for evidence to support or to refute these hypotheses and to determine if they meet proposed criteria for demonstrating sexual selection [Snowdon, C. T. (2004). Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives]. Signal content refers to patterns of acoustic features within vocalizations from which listeners might extract cues or information about the signaler. I interpret signal function, in turn, from behavioral responses of receivers and assume it has ultimate effects on the evolution and design of acoustic signals if direct fitness consequences exist. After the general review across primates, I propose orangutans as a candidate species for further evaluation of sexual selection in vocal communication. The available evidence corroborates a demonstrable relationship between sexual selection and adult male loud calls based on individual recognition, but it is necessary to obtain additional data to affirm a direct benefit to reproductive success.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Alatalo, R. V., Glynn, D., and Lundberg, A. (1990). Singing rate and female attraction in the pied flycatcher: An experiment. Anim. Behav. 39: 601–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M. (1982). Sexual selection, natural selection and quality advertisement. Biol. J. Soc. 17: 375–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual Selection, Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M. B., and Iwasa, Y. (1996). Sexual selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11: 53–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arak, A. (1983). Male-male competition and mate choice in anuran amphibians. In Bateson, P. (ed.), Mate Choice, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 181–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. C., Bjerke, T. K., Lampe, H., and Espmark, Y. (1986). Sexual response of female great tits to variation in size of males’ song repertoires. Am. Natur. 5: 395–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2003). Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302: 1234–1236.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boinksi, S. (1991). The coordination of spatial position: A field study of the vocal behavior of adult female squirrel monkeys. Anim. Behav. 41: 89–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boinksi, S. (1996). Vocal coordination of troop movement in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedi and S. sciureusi) and white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). In Norconk, M., Rosenberger, A., and Garber, P. A. (eds.), Adaptive Radiations of Neotropical Primates, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 251–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury, J., and Vehrencamp, S. (1998). Principles of Animal Communication, Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butynski, T. M., Chapman, C. A., Chapman, L. J., and Weary, D. M. (1992). Use of blue monkey “pyow” calls for long-term individual identification. Am. J. Primatol. 28: 183– 189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1981a). Distance vocalizations of Guinea baboons (Papio papio) inSenegal: An analysis of function. Behaviour 78: 283–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1981b). Uses of long-range calls during ranging by Guinea baboons. In Chiarelli, A. B., and Corruccini, R. S. (eds.), Primate Behavior and Sociobiology, Springer-Verlag, Berlin Heidelberg, New York, pp. 104–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1982). Primate vocalisations: Structural and functional approaches to understanding. Behaviour 80: 241–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1983). Bringing experimentation to the field of primate vocalisation. In Seth, P. K., and Seth, S. (eds.), Perspectives in Primate Biology, Today and Tomorrow's Printers and Publishers, New Delhi, pp. 71–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole, C. K., Dittami, J., and Leisler, B. (1984). Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol. Nature 312: 563–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole, C. K. (1986). Song repertoires and reproductive success in the great reed warbler, Acrocepahlus arundinaceus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19: 439–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole, C. K., Leisler, B., and Dittami, J. (1986). Sexual differences in the responses of captive great reed warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) to variation in song structure and repertoire size. Ethology 73: 69–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, C. A., and Weary, D. M. (1990). Variability in spider monkeys’ vocalizations may provide basis for individual recognition. Am. J. Primatol. 22: 279–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav. 28: 362–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (1982). Recognition of individuals within and between groups of free-ranging vervet monkeys. Am. Zool. 22: 519–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How Monkeys See the World, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiarello, A. G. (1995). Role of loud calls in brown howlers, Alouatta fusca. Am. J. Primatol. 36: 213–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers, D. J., and MacKinnon, J. (1977). On the behaviour of siamang after playback of their calls. Primates 18: 943–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock, T. H., and Albon, S. D. (1979). The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour 69: 145–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw, G. (1992). Song function in gibbons. Behaviour 121: 131–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw, G. (1996). Sexual selection and information content in gibbon song bouts. Ethology 102: 272–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, John Murray, London. Reprinted in 1998.

  • Darwin, C. (1876). Sexual selection in relation to monkeys. Nature 15: 18–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, R. A. (2003). The function of adult male long calls in wild orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). PhD Thesis, Duke University, Durham, NC.

  • Doty, G. V., and Welch, A. M. (2001). Advertisement call duration indicates good genes for offspring feeding rate in gray tree frogs (Hyla versicolor). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49: 150–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drickamer, L. C., Gowaty, P. A., and Holmes, C. M. (2000). Free female mate choice in house mice affects reproductive success and offspring viability and performance. Anim. Behav. 59: 371–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K., Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2002). Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: Influences of context, age and individuality. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 111: 1465–1474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, J., Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2004). Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: Acoustic features and their relation to rank, age and exhaustion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56: 140–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T., and Hauser, M. D. (1995). Vocal production in nonhuman primates: Acoustics, physiology, and functional constraints on “honest” advertisement. Am. J. Primatol. 37: 191–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forester, D. C., and Czarnowsky, R. (1985). Sexual selection in the spring peeper: Role of the advertisement call. Behaviour 92: 112–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. A. (2002). Female tactics to reduce sexual harassment in the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52: 93–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galdikas, B. M. F. (1983). The orangutan long call and snag crashing at Tanjung Puting reserve. Primates 24: 371–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galdikas, B. M. F., and Insley, S. J. (1988). The fast call of the adult male orangutan. J. Mammal. 69: 371–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gautier, J. P. (1998). La communication acoustique chez les primates. Bull. Soc. Zool. France 123: 239–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gautier, J. P., and Gautier, A. (1977). Communication in old world monkeys. In Sebeok, T. (ed.), How Animals Communicate, Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp. 890–964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giacoma, C., Zugolaro, C., and Beani, L. (1997). The advertisement calls of the green toad (Bufo viridis): Variability and role in mate choice. Herpetology 53: 454–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlander, K. (1987). Variation in the song rate of the male pied flycatcher: Causes and consequences. Anim. Behav. 35: 1037–1043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gouzoules, H., and Gouzoules, S. (2002). Primate communication: By nature honest, or by experience wise? Int. J. Primatol. 23: 821–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, S., and Marler, P. (1979). The analysis of animal communication. In Marler, P., and Vandenbergh, J. G. (eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 73–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, F. H., and Mech, L. D. (1983). Wolf pack spacing: Howling as a territory independent spacing mechanism in a territorial population. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 12: 161–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. (1993). The evolution of nonhuman primate vocalizations: Effects of phylogeny, body weight, and social context. Am. Natur. 142: 528–542.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, M. O., and Hohmann, G. M. (1984). Male loud calls in Macaca silenus and Presbytis johnii. Folia Primatol. 43: 189–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, G., MacLarnon, A., and Jones, K. E. (2002). The function of laryngeal air sacs in primates: A new hypothesis. Folia Primatol. 73: 70–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G. (1989). Comparative study of vocal communication in two Asian leaf monkeys. Folia Primatol. 52: 27–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G. (1990). Loud calls of male purple-faced langurs. Folia Primatol. 55: 200–206.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G., and Fruth, B. (1994). Structure and use of distance calls in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus). Int. J. Primatol. 15: 767–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G., and Fruth, B. (1995). Loud calls in great apes: Sex differences and social correlates. In Zimmerman E. (ed.), Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 161–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G., and Vogl, L. (1991). Loud calls of male Nilgiri langurs: Age-, individual-, and population-specific differences. Int. J. Primatol. 12: 503–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horr, D. A. (1975). The Borneo orang-utan: Population structure and dynamics in relationship to ecology and reproductive strategy. In Rosenbaum, L. A. (ed.), Primate Behavior, Academic Press, New York, pp. 307–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennions, M. D., and Petrie, M. (1997). Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences. Biol. Rev. 72: 283–327.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jorgensen, D., and French, J. (1998). Individuality but not stability in marmoset long calls. Ethology 104: 729–742.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, J. N., Winship-Ball, A., and Sim, L. (1978). Maternal discrimination of infant vocalizations in squirrel monkeys. Primates 19: 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kappeler, P., and van Schaik, C. P. (2004). Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keddy-Hector, A. C. (1992). Mate choice in non-human primates. Am. Zool. 32: 62–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinzey, W. G., and Robinson, J. G. (1983). Intergroup loud calls, range size, and spacing in Callicebus torquatus. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 60: 539–544.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkpatrick, P. (1982). Sexual selection and the evolution of female mate choice. Evolution 36: 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitchen, D. M. (2003). Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: Effect of numeric odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Anim. Behav. 67: 125–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., Fischer, J., and Cheney, D. L. (2003). Loud calls as indicators of dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 53: 374–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, J. (1974). The behaviour and ecology of wild orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Anim. Behav. 22: 3–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manson, J. H. (1995). Female mate choice in primates. Evol. Anthropol. 3: 192–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P. (1967). Animal communication signals. Science 157: 769–774.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P. (1972). Vocalizations of east African monkeys II: Black and white colobus. Behaviour 42: 175–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P., and Hobbett, L. (1975). Individuality in a long-range vocalization of wild chimpanzees. Z. Tierpsychol. 38: 97–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, A., Wrangham, R., and Clark Arcadi, A. (1999). Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? Anim. Behav. 58: 825–830.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. T., and Marshall, E. R. (1976). Gibbons and their territorial songs. Science 193: 235–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marten, K., Quine, D., and Marler, P. (1977). Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2: 291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masataka, N. (1994). Lack of correlation between body size and frequency of vocalization in young female Japanese macaques. Folia Primatol. 63: 115–118.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McComb, K. (1991). Female choice for high roaring rates in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Anim. Behav. 41: 79–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McComb, K., Pusey, A., Packer, C., and Grinnell, J. (1993). Female lions can identify potentially infanticidal males from their roars. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 252: 59–64.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C. (1984). The behavioral regulation of monogamy in gibbons (Hylobates muelleri). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 15: 225–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C. (1985a). Response of gibbons (Hylobates muelleri) to self, neighbor and stranger song duets. Int. J. Primatol. 6: 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C. (1985b). Sexual selection and adult male orangutan long calls. Anim. Behav. 33: 272–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C. (1987). Species discrimination of male song in gibbons. Am. J. Primatol. 13: 413–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C. (1988). Male gibbon singing behavior: Natural history, song variations and function. Ethology 79: 177–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C. (1990). Experimental field studies of Asian ape social systems. Int. J. Primatol. 11: 103–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., and Brandt, K. L. (1994). Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology 96: 233–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MItani, J. C., and Gros-Louis, J. (1996). Selection for acoustic individuality within the vocal repertoire of wild chimpanzees. Int. J. Primatol. 17: 569–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L., and Murdoch, M. E. (1999). Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: A reassessment. Am. J. Primatol. 47: 133–151.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., and Marler, P. (1987). Vocal communication in primates and birds: Parallels and contrasts. In Todt, D., Symxes, D., and Goldeking, P. (eds.), Primate Vocal Communication, Springer-Verlag, Berlin Heidelberg, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., and Nishida, T. (1993). Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 45: 735–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., and Stuht, J. (1998). The evolution of nonhuman primate loud calls: Acoustic adaptation for long-distance transmission. Primates 39: 171–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Møller, A. P., and Alatalo, R. V. (1999). Good-genes effects in sexual selection. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266: 85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owings, D. H., and Morton, E. S. (1998). Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, A. (2002). Sexual selection and mate choice. Int. J. Primatol. 3: 877–904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petrie, M. (1994). Improved growth and survival of offspring of peacocks with more elaborate trains. Nature 371: 598–599.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Radesater, T., Jakobsson, S., Andbjer, N., Bylin, A., and Nystrom, K. (1987). Song rate and pair formation in the willow warbler. Anim. Behav. 35: 1645–1651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raemaekers, J. J., and Raemaekers, P. M. (1985a). Field playback of loud calls to gibbons (Hylobates lar): Territorial, sex-specific, and species-specific responses. Anim. Behav. 33: 481–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raemaekers, P. M., and Raemaekers, J. J. (1985b). Long-range vocal interactions between groups of gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behaviour 95: 26–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raemaekers, J. J., Raemaekers, P. M., and Haimoff, E. H. (1984). Loud calls of the gibbon (Hylobates lar): Repertoire, organisation and context. Behaviour 91: 146– 189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendall, D., Rodman, P. S., and Emond, R. (1996). Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Anim. Behav. 51: 1007–1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, D. G., and Wiley, R. H. (1980). Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest: Implications for animal communication. Am. Natur. 115: 381–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rijksen, H. D. (1978). A Field Study on Sumatran Orangutans, H. Veenman and Zonen, Wageningen.

  • Robinson, J. G. (1979). Vocal regulation of use of space by groups of titi monkeys (Callicebus moloch). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 5: 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. G. (1981). Vocal regulation of inter- and intra-group spacing during boundary encounters in the titi monkey, Callicebus moloch. Primates 22: 161–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodman, P. S. (1973). Population composition and adaptive organization among orangutans of the Kutai Reserve. In Michael, R. P., and Crook, J. P. (eds.), Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates, Academic Press, London, pp. 171–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, D., and Hack, M. (1992). Horse signals: The sounds and scents of fury. Evol. Ecol. 6: 254–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M. J. (1988). Energy, calling, and selection. Am. Zool. 28: 885–898.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M. J. (1990). Sexual selection, sensory systems and sensory exploitation. Oxford Surv. Evol. Biol. 7: 157–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M. J. (1997). Sexual selection and mate choice. In Krebs, R., and Davies, N. B. (eds.), Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, Cambridge: Blackwell Science, pp. 179–202.

  • Schwartz, J. J. (1986). Male calling behavior and female choice in the neotropical treefrog. Ethology 73: 116–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searcy, W. A. (1992). Song repertoire and mate choice in birds. Am. Zool. 32: 71–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy, W. A., and Andersson, M. (1986). Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 17: 507–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sekulic, R. (1982). The function of howling in red howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus). Behaviour 81: 38–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sekulic, R., and Chivers, D. J. (1986). The significance of call duration in howler monkeys. Int. J. Primatol. 7: 183–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smuts, B., and Smuts, R. (1993). Male aggression and sexual coercion in nonhuman primates and other mammals. In Slater, P. J., Rosenblatt, J. S., Snowdon, C. T., and Milinski, M. (eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior, Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 1–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T. (2004). Sexual Selection and Communication. In Kappeler, P., and van Schaik, C. P. (eds.), Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 57–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T., and Cleveland, J. (1980). Individual recognition of contact calls in pygmy marmosets. Anim. Behav. 28: 717–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenbeek, R., and Assink, P. (1996). Individual differences in long-distance calls of male wild Thomas langurs (Presbystis thomasi). Folia Primatol. 69: 77–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenbeek, R., Assink, P., and Wich, S. (1999). Tenure related changes in the wild Thomas's Langurs II: Loud calls. Behaviour 136: 627–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teixidor, P., and Byrne, R. (1997). Can spider monkeys discriminate vocalizations of familiar individuals and strangers? Folia Primatol. 68: 254–264.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teixidor, P., and Byrne, R. (1999). The “whinny” of spider monkeys: Individual recognition before situational meaning. Behaviour 136: 279–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenaza, R. R. (1976). Songs, choruses, and countersinging of Kloss’ gibbons in Siberut Island, Indonesia. Z. Tierpsychol. 40: 37–52.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Utami, S. S., and Mitra Setia, T. (1995). Behavioral changes in wild male and female Sumatran Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) during and following a resident male take-over. In Nadler, R. D., Galdikas, B. M. F., Sheeran, L. K., and Rosen, N. (eds.), The Neglected Ape, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 183–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • van't Land, J. (1990). Who is calling there? The social context of adult male orang-utan long calls. Doctorandus, University of Utrecht, Utrecht.

  • Waser, P. (1975). Experimental playbacks show vocal mediation of intergroup avoidance in a forest monkey. Nature 255: 56–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waser, P., and Brown, C. H. (1986). Habitat acoustics and primate communication. Am. J. Primatol. 10: 135–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waser, P. M., and Waser, M. S. (1977). Experimental studies of primate vocalization: Specializations for long-distance propagation. Z. Tierpsychol. 43: 239–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wedell, N., and Tregenza, T. (1999). Successful fathers sire successful sons. Evolution 53: 620–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D. J., Garibaldi, B. T., and Hauser, M. D. (2001). The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Acoustic analyses and playback experiments. J. Comp. Psychol. 115: 258–271.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, A. M., Semlitsch, R. D., and Gerhardt, H. C. (1998). Call duration as an indicator of genetic quality in male gray tree frogs. Science 280: 1928–1930.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, J. M. (1987). Vocally mediated reciprocity between neighboring groups of mantled howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata palliata). Anim. Behav. 35: 1615–1627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, J. M. (1989). The effect of the location of a simulated intruder on responses to long-distance vocalizations of mantled howling monkeys. Behaviour 108: 73–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A., Assink, P. R., Becher, F., and Sterck, E. H. M. (2002). Playbacks of loud calls to wild Thomas langurs (Primates: Presbytis thomasi): The effect of familiarity. Behaviour 139: 79–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A., Koski, S., de Vries, H., and van Schaik, C. P. (2003). Individual and contextual variation in Thomas langur male loud calls. Ethology 109: 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A., and Nunn, C. L. (2002). Do male “long-distance call: Function in mate defense? A comparative study of long-distance calls in primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52: 474–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, R. H., and Richards, D. G. (1978). Physical constraints on acoustic communication: Implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 3: 69–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., and Wrangham, R. W. (2001). Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or `rank for wild chimpanzees? Anim. Behav. 61: 1203–1216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yasukawa, K., Blank, J. L., and Patterson, C. B. (1980). Song repertoires and sexual selection in the red-winged blackbird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7: 233–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ybarra, M. A. S. (1986). Loud calls of adult male red howling monkeys. Folia Primatol. 47: 204–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53: 204–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1982). The pattern of vocal signals and the information they convey. Behaviour 80: 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, E. (1995). Loud calls in nocturnal prosimians: Structure, evolution, and ontogeny. In Zimmermann, E., Newman, J. D., and Jurgens, U. (eds.), Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 47–72.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Discussions with, and observations from, Carel P. van Schaik greatly benefited this paper. In addition, I am very grateful for constructive comments from Drs. Charles Snowdon, John Mitani, and members of my doctoral committee: Drs. Susan Alberts, Ken Glander, Steve Nowicki, and Kathleen Smith. I also received helpful feedback from Duke graduate students and BAA faculty through the informal discussion forum, BEAST: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiological Topics. Lastly, I am especially indebted to Joyce Parga for inviting me to participate in the Sexual Selection symposium at ASP in Calgary 2003 and contribute to this issue of the International Journal of Primatology. A Duke Graduate School Summer Research Fellowship provided funding for my 2000 field season in Sumatra, Indonesia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roberto A. Delgado.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Delgado, R.A. Sexual Selection in the Loud Calls of Male Primates: Signal Content and Function. Int J Primatol 27, 5–25 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-005-9001-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-005-9001-4

KEY WORDS

Navigation