Introduction

Geographic distribution of species at various spatial scales may be driven by multiple factors, including historical (speciation, range shifts) and ecological (habitat selection, competition) processes (Warren et al., 2014). The occurrence of alien species during their early invasion stages may be accidental, depending on a fortuitous place of introduction rather than on their environmental demands (Simberloff & Gibbons, 2004). Moreover, as many factors act differently in novel areas (e.g. due to the enemy release effect, Torchin et al., 2003), alien organisms may thrive in different ranges of environmental conditions than in their native regions, which is difficult to predict on the basis of their ecology in previously occupied locations (Pearman et al., 2008; Medley, 2010). Furthermore, newly established populations may differ in their physiological, behavioural and ecological traits compared to previously established populations (MacMahon, 1996; Llewelyn et al., 2010) due to local adaptations or genetic drift (Bock et al., 2015). Thus, it is difficult to infer ecological processes exclusively from species distribution (Warren et al., 2014) and experimental studies are necessary to determine exact preferences of alien organisms for particular environmental variables and their combinations. Experimental approach would allow researchers to understand the local distribution of alien species, predict areas susceptible to their further expansion and organisms likely to be endangered by their appearance.

Freshwater ecosystems are much more susceptible to biological invasions than most terrestrial and marine environments due to their higher level of variability and isolation, resulting in a greater naïveté of freshwater communities and therefore stronger impacts of invaders (Ricciardi & MacIsaac, 2011). As abiotic factors in aquatic ecosystems vary across temporal and spatial scales, the ability to detect changes in them and find an optimum microhabitat is crucial for the fitness of motile animals (Lampert & Sommer, 2007). In freshwater ecosystems, the most important abiotic factors shaping the living conditions include temperature, ionic content of water and flow rate. Other important factors, such as substratum type, oxygen concentration, turbidity, light or pH, depend to a large extent on the above-mentioned conditions (Allan & Castillo, 2007; Lampert & Sommer, 2007).

Temperature shapes multiple aspects of animal functioning through its impact on metabolic rate, food availability and oxygen concentration (Hawkins, 1996). Temperature may vary vertically due to the stratification of the water column, as well as horizontally and temporally in shallow waters, affected by shading and solar radiation (Lagerspetz & Vainio, 2006). For alien species, temperature can be the most important environmental factor determining the possibility of survival, reproduction and establishment in the new area in the case of climatic differences between the donor and recipient regions (Müller & Baur, 2011). Temperature changes may also induce dispersal of alien species (Rosa et al., 2012).

Hydrodynamics, including water current and wave actions, is a crucial factor in running waters (Allan & Castillo, 2007), determining substratum quality, turbidity, oxygen content and the possibility of animal movement and settlement, as well as bringing food and infochemicals (Arnold, 1974). Water currents are also an effective way of dispersal of aquatic species (Brittain & Eikeland, 1988; Allan & Castillo, 2007), including invasive organisms (Van Riel et al., 2011).

Ionic content is particularly important for stenohaline species, but other organisms may also benefit from the presence of certain ions, e.g. calcium necessary for molluscan shells and crustacean cuticles (Lampert & Sommer, 2007). Salinity varies horizontally near river outlets to the sea and where affluents enter larger rivers of different ionic contents (Rozengurt, 1971; Grabowski et al., 2009). Raised ionic content may be beneficial for alien species adapted to brackish water and/or unstable salinity conditions (Devin & Beisel, 2007).

We studied microhabitat selection by two Ponto-Caspian gammarids Dikerogammarus villosus and Pontogammarus robustoides with regard to water salinity, temperature and current velocity. These two species have been invasive in a large part of Europe since the second half of the twentieth century, inhabiting mainly lower parts of large rivers, dam reservoirs and some lakes (Jażdżewski et al., 2002) and strongly influencing local communities through predation and competition (Dick & Platvoet, 2000; MacNeil et al., 2011). The physiological responses of these species and critical values of some environmental variables, such as temperature or salinity, have been studied (e.g. Wijnhoven et al., 2003; Dobrzycka-Krahel & Surowiec, 2011; Dobrzycka-Krahel et al., 2015), but data on their active preferences for these factors are limited. Field observations show that both species are euryecious and eurytopic, though D. villosus is often associated with deeper sites and hard substrata (Kley & Maier, 2005; Boets et al., 2010), whereas P. robustoides inhabits lentic habitats, macrophytes or very shallow sandy areas (Jażdżewski et al., 2002; Żytkowicz et al., 2008). Nevertheless, laboratory experiments have shown that both species prefer similar rocky substrata (Jermacz et al., 2015; Kobak et al., 2015). Thus, some other factors than the substratum itself must be responsible for their separation in the field. We intended to (1) determine ranges of environmental variables preferred by both species, to identify conditions under which they are most likely to occur; (2) check for the differences in habitat preferences between the species, explaining their potential co-occurrence or separation in the wild.

We hypothesized that both species would differ in their habitat preferences, which could help explain their field distribution. We expected that gammarids originating from brackish water habitats (Rozengurt, 1971) would exhibit preferences for increased ionic content in water (Piscart et al., 2005), preventing them from entering small, more freshwater affluents of large rivers (Grabowski et al., 2009). We also assumed that both species would select lentic sites (Devin & Beisel, 2007; Borza et al., 2017), but that P. robustoides, as a more common inhabitant of stagnant waters (Jażdżewski et al., 2002) would be relatively less adapted to flow. Finally, we expected that P. robustoides would be better adapted to counteract negative aspects of life in very shallow locations (Wawrzyniak-Wydrowska & Gruszka, 2005; Żytkowicz et al., 2008), such as extreme temperatures.

Materials and methods

Animals

We collected gammarids from the Włocławek Reservoir (the lower River Vistula, Central Poland, N 52°37′03″, E 19°19′37″) using hand nets and artificial substratum traps. In the laboratory, we placed them in 100-l stock tanks with constantly filtered and oxygenated water and used them in experiments 1–4 weeks after collection. In preliminary tests, we did not observe any changes in gammarid behaviour during this period in laboratory conditions. Gammarids were fed daily with frozen chironomid larvae and commercial fish food. Every week, we exchanged 30% of the water volume.

We assessed sizes of 100 randomly selected gammarids of each species with ImageJ 1.50 g software (W.S. Rasband, U.S. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA, https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/). Mean body length ± SD of the studied individuals of D. villosus was 15.5 ± 2.7 mm and that of P. robustoides: 14.9 ± 2.3 mm.

General experimental conditions

The experiments were conducted in tap water placed in tanks and aerated for at least 24 h before the test. Water quality was checked with a multimeter Multi340i (WTW GmbH, Weilheim, Germany). Except the parameters which were manipulated in a given experiment (see below), the conditions in the stock and experimental tanks were as follows: mean (± SD) temperature: 20.5 ± 1.2°C, pH 8.0 ± 0.4, conductivity 540 ± 20 µS/cm, oxygen concentration 7.8 ± 1.4 mg/l. The experiments were conducted in homogeneous indirect incandescent light (520 lx) monitored with a light meter (L-20A, Sonopan Ltd., Białystok, Poland). Tank sides were covered with Styrofoam curtains to exclude external stimuli. In all experiments, gammarids were tested in groups of 5–20 individuals (depending on the size of experimental tanks) as we assume this would better correspond to their natural behaviour in the wild, where they are usually exposed to the presence of conspecifics.

Experiment 1: salinity

Conductivity is a good measure of water salinity (Wagner et al., 2006) and has been used in other studies on amphipod distribution (Grabowski et al., 2009). We used a glass y-maze (Fig. 1) with the source tanks containing water of two different conductivities. We added marine aquarium salt (Red Sea Salt®) to reversed osmosis filtered water to obtain conductivities of 200 and 900 µS/cm in the source tanks, which resulted in the mean (±SD) values of 456 ± 40 and 831 ± 34 µS/cm in the y-arms, corresponding to those observed in small tributaries and large rivers in Europe, respectively (Grabowski et al. 2009). The conductivity in the mixing zone was intermediate. The conductivity measured at the sampling site during the collection of gammarids (653 µS/cm) was also intermediate between the y-arms. To control for the effect of acclimation to particular conditions, we tested two groups of gammarids: acclimated in the stock tanks for 3 weeks to the conductivities of 450 and 850 µS/cm.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Y-maze used to test responses of gammarids to salinity (Experiment 1). Dashed lines depict zones of the tank

We established the flow (0.5 l/min.) and placed 10 gammarids into the mixing zone of the y-maze. Each experimental trial consisted of 5 min. of adaptation and 20 min. of the test. Gammarid behaviour was recorded using an IP video camera (SNB-6004, Samsung, South Korea) placed above the tank. The experiment was replicated 15 times. We switched the high and low conductivity y-arms and rinsed the tank with tap water between the replicates.

We analysed gammarid behaviour (location in the tank and activity) using Noldus Ethovision XT® 10.1 software. All 10 individuals from a single replicate were treated as a unit and average parameter values for each replicate were used as data points in the further analyses. We determined (1) percentage of time spent by gammarids in each y-maze zone (to check their selectivity) and (2) gammarid activity expressed as a percentage of time spent on moving while present in the zone (as the increased/decreased activity could reflect a continuous search for an optimum location or a freezing response, respectively). We conducted the following analyses:

  1. (1)

    To check gammarid preferences for particular salinities, we calculated differences (D) between the percentages of time spent in each y-arm (high salinity arm—low salinity arm):

    D = (HS − LS)/(HS + LS)

    where HS and LS are the percentages of time spent in the high and low salinity y-arm, respectively. This formula takes into account the difference relative to the overall time spent in the y-arms in a given replicate. Then, we compared the calculated mean difference values in particular treatments with a theoretical value of 0 (indicating no selectivity) using sequential Bonferroni-corrected one-sample t-tests. This approach allowed us to counteract the problem of the lack of independence between the times spent by gammarids in various parts of the arena.

  2. (2)

    To check whether gammarids under some conditions reduced their visits to both y-arms, staying in the mixing zone (which would indicate suboptimal conditions in both y-arms), we compared percentages of time spent in the mixing zone using a two-way ANOVA with species and acclimation conditions (low or high conductivity) as factors. This variable could not be directly compared with the y-arms due to their different sizes and shapes.

  3. (3)

    To compare gammarid activity between the y-arms, we used a three-way mixed model ANOVA with species and acclimation conditions as between-subject factors and y-arm conductivity (low or high) as a within-subject factor.

Experiment 2: current velocity

We used a cross-shaped flow-through tank with flow generated by a water pump (Aquarium Systems NewJet 4500) and regulated by ball valves (Fig. 2). We set up a desired current velocity on the basis of 20 measurements made with an ultrasonic gauge (Greyline Instruments Inc., Massena, NY, USA). We used water velocities of 0 (control), 5, 10, 15, and 30 cm/s, covering the entire range observed in the reservoir (Gierszewski, 2006), from which the animals were collected. The gammarids could enter the main current zone or seek refuge in the stagnant water zones. Within the current zone, they could stay in the flow or adhere to one of the mesh barriers (upstream or downstream). The end walls of the stagnant water zones were also equipped with mesh barriers (Fig. 2) to make them similar in this respect to the current zones.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Flow-through tank used to test gammarid responses to water flow (Experiment 2). Dashed lines depict zones of the tank

The tank bottom was devoid of substratum to enable the recognition of animals by the video analysis software. Although this might be regarded as an artificial situation for gammarids, it should be noted that they could utilize mesh barriers, located at the ends of all the zones (Fig. 2), as suitable shelters. Alien gammarids are “sit and wait” animals, which usually stay in the shelters, but sometimes they do migrate, e.g. to find better food areas or mating partners as well as to avoid predators or competitors (Kobak et al., 2016). In such situations, they have to expose themselves to the local flow conditions. Our experimental design can help determine how various current velocities affect the probability of entering the flow area and active relocations of the studied species.

After establishing an appropriate flow, we put 5 gammarids in the main current zone and recorded their behaviour for 30 min. (following a 5-min. adaptation period) with a video camera located above the tank. The experiment was replicated 10–15 times for each treatment.

We analysed gammarid behaviour (location in the tank and activity) using Noldus Ethovision XT® 10.1. Values for all 5 individuals in a single replicate were averaged and these average values were used as data points in the further analyses. Direct comparisons among the tank zones were not possible due the lack of independence among the percentage times spent by gammarids in various parts of the tank and different sizes of the zones. Therefore, we compared gammarid behaviour in the presence of flow with their behaviour in the control, stagnant water conditions in the same tank. We used two-way ANOVAs with species and current velocity as factors to test the following response variables:

  1. (1)

    The “active” preference of gammarids for flowing water associated with counteracting the water current (time spent actively in the flow), indicated by the percentage of time spent in the main current zone, including the upstream mesh but excluding the downstream mesh.

  2. (2)

    Passive stay in the flow, when the gammarids did not resist the water current and drifted downstream, indicated by the percentage of time spent on the downstream mesh.

  3. (3)

    Tendency to seek protection from the excessive flow, shown by the percentage of time spent in the stagnant water zones.

  4. (4)

    Tendency to occupy the upstream areas, indicated by the distance between the gammarids located in the main current zone and the upstream mesh (with lower values indicating a greater affinity for upstream locations).

  5. (5)

    Gammarid activity (percentage of time spent on movement in the main current zone relative to the total time spent in this zone, including both mesh barriers), reflecting their ability to withstand the flow (we could expect a decrease in the activity of animals unable to counteract the water current);

  6. (6)

    Direction choice, shown as the difference between the percentages of time spent on the upstream and downstream movement in the main current zone.

Experiment 3: temperature

We used a thermal gradient (4.7–34.2°C) with 12 compartments (Fig. 3). Two terminal compartments were separated securely with aluminium (enabling good heat transduction) barriers from the rest of the tank. We equipped one of them with an aquarium cooler Teco R20 (Teco S.l.r., Ravenna, Italy) and the other with a heater, set to 0 and 40°C, respectively. The remaining, middle part of the tank contained 9 glass barriers raised 1 cm above the bottom, leaving a common space for free gammarid movement. Such a design allowed temperature differences to be sustained along the gradient. Temperature in each compartment was constantly measured with electronic thermometers to the nearest 0.1°C. The barriers, their glass guides on the tank walls and thermometer sensors on the bottom served as shelters for gammarids, which allowed the effect of crowding in the corners of the gradient ends to be avoided.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Thermal gradient used to test gammarid responses to temperature (Experiment 3)

To check the effect of season on gammarid preferences, we collected and tested them in July (water temperature ca. 20°C) and November (ca. 10°C). Moreover, to control for the effect of acclimation conditions, we acclimated gammarids collected in each season for two weeks at 10 or 20°C. The animals during acclimation were initially placed at the field temperature, which, if needed, was gradually changed at a rate of ca. 1°C per h.

We put 20 gammarids into the gradient compartment with the temperature closest to their acclimation conditions to avoid an initial thermal shock. Gammarids are highly mobile and in preliminary trials dispersed evenly in the tank with no thermal gradient after 24 h. After 24 h, we counted gammarids in the compartments. Preliminary trials have shown that such results were comparable with those from a continuous observation, so we gave up the latter, more laborious procedure. The experiment was replicated 10 times. For each replicate, we calculated the mean temperature selected by gammarids (MST):

$${\text{MST}} = \sum \left( {N_{i} * \, T_{i} } \right)/\sum N_{i},$$

where N i —the number of animals in compartment i; T i —the temperature in compartment i.

We conducted the following analyses:

  1. (1)

    To check the effect of species, season (summer or autumn) and acclimation conditions (10 or 20°C) on MST, we used a three-way ANOVA;

  2. (2)

    To check whether gammarids selected temperatures higher or lower than their acclimation temperature, we compared MST values with corresponding acclimation temperatures for each treatment using sequential Bonferroni-corrected one-sample t-tests.

General statistical remarks

Data were log-transformed prior to analyses to meet the normality and homoscedasticity assumptions (checked with Shapiro–Wilk and Levene tests, respectively) except for the differences between the y-arms (Experiment 1) and movement directions (Experiment 2), which could assume negative values. However, in these two cases, the normality assumption was met without any transformation. The sphericity assumption was confirmed with a Mauchly test for within-subject factors. Significant ANOVA effects for factors with more than two levels were followed by sequential-Bonferroni-corrected t-tests as a post hoc procedure.

Results

Experiment 1: salinity

Dikerogammarus villosus always selected the low conductivity arm of the y-maze, whereas Pontogammarus robustoides did not discriminate between different conductivities, as indicated by significant one-sample t-tests (Fig. 4A; Table S1). Moreover, compared to P. robustoides and the low acclimation conductivity treatment, D. villosus acclimated to higher conductivity spent more time in the mixing zone (Fig. 4A, a significant species x acclimation conductivity interaction in ANOVA: F 1, 55 = 19.3, P < 0.001; see Table S2 for the full results of statistical tests) and increased its activity (Fig. 4B, a significant species x acclimation conductivity x y-arm conductivity interaction in ANOVA: F 1, 55 = 4.9, P = 0.031).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Responses of gammarids acclimated to various conditions to water conductivity (Experiment 1): A Mean (± SE) percentage of time spent by gammarids in the y-maze arms of different conductivities (the remaining time was spent in the mixing zone); B Mean gammarid activity (percentage of time spent on moving) in the y-maze arms of different conductivities. N values show the numbers of replicates. Asterisks denote statistically significant differences in gammarid behaviour between the y-arms. Different letters above the bars indicate statistically significant differences between particular treatments with respect to the time spent by gammarids in the mixing zone (a) and gammarid activity in the y-arms (b)

Experiment 2: current velocity

The distribution of gammarids in the flow-through tank zones was affected by current velocity (significant main effects in ANOVA: F 4, 113 = 7.1, P < 0.001; F 4, 113 = 10.4, P < 0.001; F 4, 113 = 2.8, P = 0.027 for the main current & upstream mesh, downstream mesh and stagnant water zones, respectively; see Table S3 for the full ANOVA results), but not by species. Gammarids never spent more time in flowing water (i.e. in the main current zone or on the upstream mesh) than in the corresponding area under no flow conditions (Fig. 5A). Time spent in flowing water was reduced at the velocities of 15–30 cm/s. At 15 cm/s, gammarids more often clung to the downstream mesh, whereas at 30 cm/s they moved to the stagnant water zones (Fig. 5A).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Responses of gammarids to current velocity (Experiment 2): A Mean (± SE) percentage of time spent by gammarids in various tank zones; B mean distance between the gammarids occurring in the main current zone and the upstream end of the tank; C Mean duration of the upstream and downstream movements of gammarids in the main current zone. N values show the numbers of replicates. Water velocities differing significantly from one another with regard to gammarid behaviour are labelled with different letters (for the times spent in the tank zones shown in panel A, uppercase, lowercase and Greek letters stand for the main current and upstream mesh combined, downstream mesh and stagnant water zones, respectively)

In the faster flow (15–30 cm/s), both species were located further downstream than at the lower velocities (Fig. 5B, the main effect of current velocity in ANOVA: F 4, 113 = 5.5, P < 0.001). Moreover, D. villosus was found further downstream than P. robustoides irrespective of the current velocity (the main effect of species in ANOVA: F 1, 113 = 9.3, P = 0.003).

Dikerogammarus villosus was more active than P. robustoides (Fig. 5C) and both species were more active at 30 cm/s than in the other treatments, resulting in significant main effects of species and current velocity in ANOVA (F 1, 113 = 16.2 and F 4, 113 = 10.7, respectively, P < 0.001). Dikerogammarus villosus spent significantly more time on the upstream movement than on moving along the flow direction at 30 cm/s, whereas P. robustoides exhibited no such differences (Fig. 5C), which resulted in a significant species x current velocity interaction in ANOVA F 4, 113 = 2.6, P = 0.041).

Experiment 3: temperature

Gammarids selected higher temperatures than those to which they had been acclimated, except D. villosus acclimated in autumn to a low temperature and P. robustoides acclimated in summer to a high temperature, as shown by one-sample t-tests (Fig. 6; Table S4). It should be noted that considerable numbers of individuals, particularly of D. villosus acclimated to a high temperature, moved and stayed in the warmest compartment (Fig. 7), at a temperature of 34 ± 1.8°C (Fig. 3). In general, D. villosus exhibited a stronger tendency to select extreme temperatures (Fig. 7).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Mean temperature (±SE) selected by gammarids acclimated to various temperatures in various seasons (Experiment 3). Acclimation temperatures are denoted by horizontal-dashed lines. N is the number of replicates. Asterisks indicate significant departures of the selected temperatures from acclimation temperatures in particular treatments. Different letters above the bars indicate treatments differing significantly from one another with respect to the selected temperature

Fig. 7
figure 7

Mean percentages (±SE) of gammarids occupying particular compartments of the thermal gradient after acclimation to various temperatures in various seasons (Experiment 3). Mean temperatures in particular compartments are shown in Fig. 3

The comparison of temperatures selected by gammarids in different treatments revealed significant two-way interactions between all tested factors in ANOVA (F 1, 72 = 14.2, P < 0.001 and F 1, 72 = 9.8, P = 0.002 and F 1, 72 = 7.3, P = 0.009, for species x acclimation temperature, species x season and acclimation temperature x season interactions, respectively; see Table S5 for the full statistical details). Individuals acclimated to a high temperature selected warmer areas than those acclimated to a low temperature except P. robustoides in summer (Fig. 6). Significant seasonal differences were only shown by P. robustoides, which selected warmer water in autumn. Moreover, D. villosus acclimated to a high temperature in summer selected warmer areas than P. robustoides and this tendency was reversed in the animals acclimated to a low temperature in autumn.

Discussion

Experiment 1: salinity

Contrary to our expectations, the tested gammarids selected the low conductivity y-arm (D. villosus) or did not exhibit any selectivity (P. robustoides) (Fig. 4A). Even the individuals of D. villosus adapted to high conductivity more often visited the low conductivity zone, though they significantly reduced time spent in both y-arms in favour of the mixing zone and increased their activity (Fig. 4B), suggesting that neither of the y-arms was optimal for them. Such behaviour contradicts the hypothesis that lower salinity is the reason for the avoidance of small, low-order streams by the Ponto-Caspian amphipods, which allows the native species to find a refuge in such areas, whereas the aliens thrive in large rivers with anthropogenically increased ionic content (Grabowski et al., 2009).

Ponto-Caspian gammarids are good osmoregulators within a wide salinity range (Bruijs et al., 2001; Dobrzycka-Krahel & Surowiec, 2011; Dobrzycka-Krahel et al., 2015) due to their evolution in the highly variable environment of Black Sea limans (Rozengurt, 1971). The studied species can inhabit waters of much higher salinity than those tested in our study, up to 6 PSU (Dobrzycka-Krahel & Surowiec, 2011; Dobrzycka-Krahel et al., 2015), which roughly corresponds to 12000 μS/cm (Wagner et al., 2006). Pontogammarus robustoides has greater salinity resistance, surviving even in fully saline water of 34 PSU (Santagata et al., 2008) than D. villosus tolerating up to 20 PSU (Bruijs et al., 2001). This difference between the species may explain the affinity of D. villosus for lower conductivity in our experiment. Perhaps under conditions of highly fluctuating salinity, such a preference might help gammarids locate optimum microhabitats, e.g. by preventing their seaward migrations. Similarly, estuarine copepods exhibit counter-current migrations keeping them at a constant position within an estuary against the net seaward water movement (Shang et al., 2008). A direct response to conductivity gradients, observed in our study, might play such a role in the field.

Nevertheless, even despite their preferences, alien gammarids may have an advantage over their native counterparts due to the anthropogenically increased salinity of the large rivers they live in (Grabowski et al., 2009). However, an open question is why D. villosus avoids small affluents, which it should prefer with regard to their lower salinity. Maybe a fast water flow or variable (commonly lower) temperature in small streams (Allan & Castillo, 2007) form such a barrier (see the discussion on these factors below), perhaps in association with low ionic content. Dikerogammarus villosus is less resistant to extreme values of other factors (e.g. temperature) in purely fresh water than in an oligohaline environment (Wijnhoven et al., 2003). On the other hand, it thrives in alpine lakes with low ionic content (Bącela-Spychalska et al., 2013). Thus, in order to provide an unambiguous explanation of this phenomenon further studies are needed.

Experiment 2: current velocity

The gammarids showed no affinity for any of the tested flow velocities (Fig. 5A), which may limit their distribution to lentic areas or structured habitats (Jażdżewski et al., 2002; Żytkowicz et al., 2008; Grabowski et al., 2009; Borza et al., 2017). Avoidance of fast flowing waters distinguishes invasive amphipods from those staying within their native ranges (Devin & Beisel, 2007). Native European gammarids have been observed to move actively upstream (Dennert et al., 1969; Hultin, 1971) and prefer moderate flow velocities up to ca. 10 cm/s (Hughes, 1970). This difference between native and alien gammarids corroborates their current distribution in Europe, where native species find refuges in smaller, fast flowing rivers and streams (Grabowski et al., 2009).

Nevertheless, the alien gammarids in our study withstood flow velocities up to 10 cm/s with no significant changes in behaviour and were negatively affected by a current velocity of ≥15 cm/s. In the field, animals can use stones, woody debris or plants as flow refuges. Both studied species are associated with such objects (Kley et al., 2009; Jermacz et al., 2015) and showed the ability to utilize shelters (i.e. the downstream mesh) to stay within the flow zone at a flow of 15 cm/s. At a current velocity of 30 cm/s, gammarids changed their behaviour, moving to the stagnant water refuges (Fig. 5A). Thus, they were still able to counteract the flow and actively search for more suitable sites. The velocities used in our study cover the range typically observed in our sampling area (Gierszewski, 2006). Thus, we can assume that we comprised the entire range of gammarid responses to flow, from the neutral behaviour at low velocities to the avoidance of strong currents.

Dikerogammarus villosus seemed more affected by a high current water velocity than P. robustoides, as shown by its greater downward displacement (Fig. 5B) and higher change in activity (Fig. 5C). This difference between the species is surprising, considering that D. villosus is regarded as more associated with running waters (Bij de Vaate et al., 2002) than P. robustoides. Perhaps the adaptations of P. robustoides to commonly occupied sandy areas (Wawrzyniak-Wydrowska & Gruszka, 2005; Żytkowicz et al., 2008), which are poorly sheltered and exposed to wave actions, allowed it to counteract the water flow in our experiment. In a field study, D. villosus was found to be the least rheotolerant compared to D. haemobaphes and D. bispinosus and the most associated with structured habitats offering protection against flow (Borza et al., 2017). Our experiment confirmed its ability to locate and utilize lentic microhabitats which can help it thrive in flowing waters.

The significantly longer duration of the upstream movement compared with the downstream relocation of D. villosus at the highest current velocity (Fig. 5C) may indicate a compensation for passive drifting. Given the greater downstream displacement of D. villosus under these conditions (Fig. 5B), such behaviour would reduce passive relocation and allow an animal to stay in the same position.

Many riverine organisms, including Gammarus spp., actively enter the water column to utilize the benefits of the flow (Dennert et al., 1969; Brittain & Eikeland, 1988). They move upstream to compensate for passive drifting (Dennert et al., 1969; Hughes, 1970; Hultin, 1971), to approach an odour source (e.g. food) (Bousfield, 1978; Peckarsky & Dodson, 1980; Pasternak et al., 2004) or to find a target of a long-distance migration (Arnold, 1974). They also drift downstream to avoid predators (Peckarsky & Dodson 1980; Lancaster, 1990) or deteriorating conditions (Waters, 1972; Brittain & Eikeland, 1988). Nevertheless, our study suggests that, although the tested Ponto-Caspian gammarids tolerate moderate flow velocities, they do not exhibit any positive rheotaxis.

Experiment 3: temperature

Both tested species generally preferred warm water, selecting higher temperatures than those to which they had been acclimated (Fig. 6). Particularly, D. villosus acclimated to a high temperature was often noted in the warmest part of the gradient (Fig. 7), at c.a. 35°C (Fig. 3). Such behaviour may seem maladaptive, as they would eventually die staying in such conditions for a longer time (personal observations). However, animals seldom encounter such extreme temperatures in the field and therefore the selection of the highest available temperature would allow them to find optimum thermal conditions.

Both native European and Ponto-Caspian gammarids tolerate a wide temperature range, up to ca. 30°C (Wijnhoven et al., 2003). Nevertheless, the gammarids did select particular temperatures in our study. Acclimation temperature strongly affected the choice of D. villosus, whereas the responses of P. robustoides depended more on the season (Fig. 6). Pontogammarus robustoides seemed to prefer warm water in the colder season and to avoid excessive heat in summer, suggesting a behavioural temperature regulation. Thus, this species appears to be better adapted to regulate its thermal conditions than D. villosus. Extreme shifts in temperature are usually found in shallow, unsheltered bottoms often inhabited by P. robustoides (Wawrzyniak-Wydrowska & Gruszka, 2005; Żytkowicz et al., 2008), whereas D. villosus lives in deeper waters (Kley & Maier, 2005; own observations in the sampling area) and/or on stony bottoms (Kley et al., 2009; Boets et al., 2010) where temperature regimes may be more constant. The effect of season (independent of acclimation) on thermal behaviour has also been observed in crabs (Cuculescu et al., 1998) and crayfish (Layne et al., 1985), but not in an isopod Asellus aquaticus, varying its thermal behaviour only in response to acclimation temperature (Lagerspetz & Bowler, 1993), like D. villosus in our study.

Temperature selection was observed in many benthic and planktonic crustaceans (Lagerspetz & Vainio, 2006). Usually, it is based on changes in activity and movement speed rather than on a directional taxis (Lagerspetz & Vainio, 2006). Temperature in the aquatic environment may vary at a microscale, particularly in shallow near-shore areas, due to variable shading by the riparian canopy, floating plant leaves and submerged shelters as well as with the circadian cycle (Pokorny & Kvet, 2003; Dallas & Rivers-Moore, 2011). Thus, the ability to select appropriate temperature greatly increases the fitness of aquatic animals. Moreover, as predatory habits of D. villosus strongly depend on temperature, this factor also shapes its effects on aquatic communities (Van der Velde et al., 2009). Maazouzi et al. (2011) found that the ability of D. villosus to adapt physiologically to high temperature was lower than that of a native species. However, in the light of our results, the alien species seem to use another strategy and adjust their ambient temperature behaviourally.

Temperature also depends on the river size. In small streams, it is more variable and affected by external factors to a greater extent than in large rivers. Small tributaries with a dense riparian canopy are colder than wider rivers, less affected by their shoreline (Allan & Castillo, 2007). Perhaps this may be one of the factors preventing alien gammarids, shown in our study to prefer warm waters, from entering smaller affluents, in which they have never been found so far (Grabowski et al., 2009).

Conclusions

In the field, multiple factors act simultaneously, interacting with each other and modifying their impacts on organisms. Therefore, it is difficult to account for interactive effects that occur in the field, and to extrapolate laboratory results directly to nature. However, our experimental study under controlled conditions is useful because it suggests cause–effect relationships between particular factors and animal responses.

We did not confirm gammarid affinity for increased ionic content. Thus, their avoidance of less saline small streams in favour of large rivers may partially depend on temperature or current velocity barriers, as suggested by our experiments. Consistent with our hypothesis, neither of the species exhibited preferences for flowing water, though they showed considerable capacity for dealing with moderate water velocities. However, contrary to our expectations, D. villosus was less resistant to the water flow than P. robustoides. Finally, in accordance with our hypothesis, P. robustoides selected more constant, seasonally dependent temperatures (cooler in summer, warmer in autumn), which may be beneficial in shallow areas often inhabited by this species.

The observed gammarid responses are likely to lead to their occurrence in warm, slowly flowing waters in the field. Given their distribution in the wild (Grabowski et al., 2009), low ionic content seems less important in their habitat selection, though, according to our study, a decrease in this parameter may also be beneficial for the studied species. Nevertheless, the ability to select an optimal microhabitat using thermal, hydrodynamic and salinity cues helps them survive under spatially and temporally variable conditions, which may contribute to their invasive potential. Relatively low selectivity with regard to water flow and, in the case of P. robustoides, salinity predestines them to live in a wide range of aquatic habitats.

Contrary to our expectations, we did not find strong differences in habitat preferences between the studied species. Even if they differed with regard to mechanisms of reactions, e.g. in the case of temperature, the final effect of their behaviour was similar (e.g. occupation of warmer areas). The similarity between the species, including their similar substratum preferences (Jermacz et al., 2015; Kobak et al., 2015), could potentially lead to strong interspecific competition and displacement. However, another study has demonstrated their variable depth preferences (with P. robustoides selecting shallower locations), which is likely to contribute to their separation and decrease competitive tensions in the field (Kobak et al., 2017).