Skip to main content
Log in

The interplay among gestures, discourse, and diagrams in students’ geometrical reasoning

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explores interactions with diagrams that are involved in geometrical reasoning; more specifically, how students publicly make and justify conjectures through multimodal representations of diagrams. We describe how students interact with diagrams using both gestural and verbal modalities, and examine how such multimodal interactions with diagrams reveal their reasoning. We argue that when limited information is given in a diagram, students make use of gestural and verbal expressions to compensate for those limitations as they engage in making and proving conjectures. The constraints of a diagram, gestures and linguistic systems are semiotic resources that students may use to engage in geometrical reasoning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The word modality is often used to refer to each of the semiotic systems used in communication; thus the expression “multimodal representation” used above. Modality is also used in linguistics with a different meaning. Particularly, in systemic linguistics, modality is used to name one of the systems with which speakers construct relationships with their audience. This is the sense with which it is used in this section. Elsewhere in the paper we may use modality in the first, semiotic, sense. The context will help clarify what is the usage alluded to.

  2. All the names of the teachers and students in this paper are pseudonyms.

References

  • Arzarello, F. (2006). Semiosis as a multimodal process. Revista Latinoamericana de Investigación en Matemática Educativa, 9(1), 267–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arzarello, F., Paola, D., Robutti, O., & Sabena, C. (2009). Gestures as semiotic resources in the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 97–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, C., Burrill, G., Cummins, J., Kanold, T., & Malloy, C. (1998). Geometry: Integration, applications, connections. New York: Glencoe-Mc Graw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18(1), 32–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Common Core Standards Initiative [CCSI]. (2010). Common core state standards for mathematics. Retrieved from Common Core Standards Initiative website: http://corestandards.org/assets/CCSSI_Math%20Standards.pdf.

  • Cook, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 211–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duval, R. (1995). Geometrical pictures: Kinds of representation and specific processings. In R. Sutherland & J. Mason (Eds.), Exploiting mental imagery with computers in mathematics education (pp. 142–157). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1993). The theory of figural concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 24, 139–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Singer, M. A. (2003). From children‘s hands to adults’ ear: Gesture’s role in the learning process. Development psychology, 39(3), 509–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1998). The situativity of knowing, learning, and research. American Psychologist, 53(1), 5–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Matthiessen, C. (2004). An introduction to functional grammar. London: Hodder Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, P. (2004). Interaction with diagrams and the making of reasoned conjectures in geometry. Zentralblatt für Didaktik der Mathematik, 36(5), 129–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, P. (2006). Teaching geometry with problems: Negotiating instructional situations and mathematical tasks. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37(4), 313–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, P., & Brach, C. (2006). Proving and proof in high school geometry: What is ‘it’ that is going on for students and how do they make sense of it? Cognition and Instruction, 24(1), 73–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, P., Chen, C.-L., Weiss, M., González, G., Nachlieli, T., Hamlin, M., et al. (2009). “Doing proofs” in geometry classrooms. In D. Stylianou, M. Blanton, & E. Knuth (Eds.), Teaching and learning proof across the grades (pp. 250–268). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laborde, C. (2005). The hidden role of diagrams in students’ construction of meaning in geometry. In J. Kilpartrick, C. Hoyles, O. Skovsmose, & P. Valero (Eds.), Meaning in mathematics education (pp. 159–179). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. (2009). Learning to mean mathematically. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 16(3), 281–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mariotti, M. A. (1995). Images and concepts in geometrical reasoning. In R. Sutherland & J. Mason (Eds.), Exploiting mental imagery with computers in mathematics education. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. R., & Rose, D. (2003). Working with discourse: Meaning beyond the clause. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maschietto, M., & Bartolini Bussi, M. G. (2009). Working with artefacts: Gestures, drawings and speech in the construction of the mathematical meaning of the visual pyramid. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 143–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1998). Speech and gesture integration. New Direction for Child Development, 79, 11–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • NCTM. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., & Tierney, C. (2001). Children creating ways to represent changing situations: On the development of homogeneous spaces. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 45, 67–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., Tierney, C., & Wright, T. (1998). Body motion and graphing. Cognition and Instruction, 16(2), 119–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. (2004). Do real numbers really move? Language, thought, and gesture: The embodied cognitive foundations of mathematics. In F. Iida, R. Pfeifer, L. Steels, & Y. Kuniyashi (Eds.), Embodied artificial intelligence (pp. 54–73). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, M. C. (1998). Language socialization in the mathematics classroom: Discourse practices and mathematical thinking. In M. Lampert & M. Blunk (Eds.), Talking mathematics in school: Studies of teaching and learning (pp. 17–55). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pozzer-Ardenghi, L., & Roth, W.-M. (2005). Photographs in lectures: Gestures as meaning-making resources. Linguistics and Education, 15, 275–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Presmeg, N. C. (2001). Visualization and affect in nonroutine problem solving. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 3(4), 289–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2009). Why do gestures matter? Sensuous cognition and the palpability of mathematical meanings. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 111–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Lawless, D. (2002a). Scientific investigations, metaphorical gestures, and the emergence of abstract scientific concepts. Learning and Instruction, 12, 285–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Lawless, D. (2002b). When up is down and down is up: Body orientation, proximity, and gestures as resources. Language in Society, 31, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Welzel, M. (2001). From activity to gestures and scientific language. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(1), 103–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (1998). On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one. Educational Researcher, 27(2), 4–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2001). There is more to discourse than meets the ears: Looking at thinking as communicating to learn more about mathematical learning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 46, 13–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2008). Thinking as communicating: Human development, the growth of discourses, and mathematizing. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zack, V., & Graves, B. (2001). Making mathematical meaning through dialogue: “Once you think of it, the Z minus three seems pretty weird”. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 46, 229–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Patricio Herbst.

Additional information

The research reported in this study was done with the support of a National Science Foundation CAREER grant, REC 0133619 to the second author. All opinions are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Foundation.

Appendix: activity worksheet of the intervention lesson

Appendix: activity worksheet of the intervention lesson

figure a

There are six lines on the paper and some of their intersections are not visible.

  1. 1.

    Would it be possible for somebody to determine the measures of all the angles formed by those lines, considering that not all angles can be measured? Explain.

  2. 2.

    What is the total number of different angle measures that one would need to determine? Explain.

  3. 3.

    How many of those angle measures would be impossible to find unless one could extend the lines beyond the screen limits? Explain.

  4. 4.

    What is the minimum number of angles that one would have to measure before being able to say “I know all the angle measures”? Explain.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chen, CL., Herbst, P. The interplay among gestures, discourse, and diagrams in students’ geometrical reasoning. Educ Stud Math 83, 285–307 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-012-9454-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-012-9454-2

Keywords

Navigation