Skip to main content
Log in

Intuitive nonexamples: the case of triangles

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper we examine the possibility of differentiating between two types of nonexamples. The first type, intuitive nonexamples, consists of nonexamples which are intuitively accepted as such. That is, children immediately identify them as nonexamples. The second type, non-intuitive nonexamples, consists of nonexamples that bear a significant similarity to valid examples of the concept, and consequently are more often mistakenly identified as examples. We describe and discuss these notions and present a study regarding kindergarten children’s grasp of nonexamples of triangles.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Kindergarten children were labeled K1-K65.

References

  • Attneave, F. (1957). Transfer of experience with a class schema to identification of patterns and shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 81–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Battista, M. T. (2007). The development of geometric and spatial thinking. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 843–908). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bills, L., Dreyfus, T., Mason, J., Tsamir, P., Watson, A., & Zaslavsky, O. (2006). Exemplification in mathematics education. In J. Novotna, H. Moraova, M. Kratka, & N. Stehlikova (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th PME International Conference (vol. 1, pp. 126–154). Czech Republic: PME.

  • Burger, W., & Shaughnessy, J. (1986). Characterizing the van Hiele levels of development in geometry. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 17(1), 31–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H. (2003). Teaching and learning geometry. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and standards for school mathematics (pp. 151–178). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D., Swaminathan, S., Hannibal, M., & Sarama, J. (1999). Young children’s concepts of shape. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(2), 192–212. doi:10.2307/749610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M., & Carpenter, J. (1980). The effects of non-examples in geometrical concept acquisition. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 11(2), 259–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1993). The interaction between the formal, the algorithmic and the intuitive components in a mathematical activity. In R. Biehler, R. Scholz, R. Strässer, & B. Winkelmann (Eds.), Didactics of Mathematics as a Scientific Discipline (pp. 231–245). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannibal, M. (1999). Young children’s developing understanding of geometric shapes. Teaching Children Mathematics, 5(6), 353–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasegawa, J. (1997). Concept formation of triangles and quadrilaterals in the second grade. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 32, 157–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hershkowitz, R. (1989). Visualization in geometry – two sides of the coin. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 11(1), 61–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hershkowitz, R. (1990). Psychological aspects of learning geometry. In P. Nesher & J. Kilpatrick (Eds.), Mathematics and cognition (pp. 70–95). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hershkowitz, R., & Vinner, S. (1983). The role of critical and non-critical attributes in the concept image of geometrical concepts. In R. Hershkowitz (Ed.), Proceedings of the 7th PME International Conference (pp. 223–228). Rehovot, Israel: Weizmann Institute of Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. (1980). Feature frequency and hypothesis testing in the acquisition of rule-governed concepts. Memory & Cognition, 8, 297–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klausmeier, H., & Feldman, K. (1975). Effects of a definition and a varying number of examples and nonexamples on concept attainment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 174–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klausmeier, H., & Sipple, T. (1980). Learning and teaching concepts. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linchevsky, L., Vinner, S., & Karsenty, R. (1992). To be or not to be minimal? Student teachers’ views about definitions in geometry. In W. Geeslin, & K. Graham (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th PME International Conference (vol. 2, pp. 48–55). New Hampshire, MA: University of New Hampshire.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. (1989). Categorization and naming in children. Massachusetts: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E., & Watchtel, G. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meaning of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKinney, C., Larkins, A., Ford, M., & Davis III, J. (1983). The effectiveness of three methods of teaching social studies concepts to fourth-grade students: an aptitude-treatment interaction study. American Educational Research Journal, 20, 663–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, O., & Jansson, L. (1987). Sequencing examples and non-examples to facilitate concept attainment. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18(2), 112–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 382–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1973). Natural Categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. (1975). Family resemblances: studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 773–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B., & Hershkowitz, R. (1999). Prototypes: brakes or levers in learning the function concept? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 362–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaughnessy, J., & Burger, W. (1985). Spadework prior to deduction in geometry. Mathematics Teacher, 78(6), 419–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E., & Medin, D. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E., Shoben, E., & Rips, L. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: a featural model for semantic decisions. Psychological Review, 81, 214–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Hiele, P. M., & van Hiele, D. (1958). A method of initiation into geometry. In H. Freudenthal (Ed.), Report on methods of initiation into geometry. Groningen: Walters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinner, S. (1991). The role of definitions in the teaching and learning of mathematics. In D. Tall (Ed.), Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 65–81). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinner, S., & Hershkowitz, R. (1980). Concept images and common cognitive paths in the development of some simple geometric concepts. In R. Karplus (Ed.), Proceedings of the 4th PME International Conference, 177–184.

  • Watson, A., & Mason, J. (2005). Mathematics as a Constructive Activity: Learners generating examples. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. (1999). The dubbin ceremony revisited: Object naming and categorization in infancy and early childhood. In D. Medin, & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S., & Braun, I. (2005). Consistent (but not variable) names as invitations to form object categories: new evidence from 12-month-old infants. Cognition, 95(3), B59–B68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. (1986). Feature frequency and the use of negative instances in a geometric task. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 17, 130–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. (1990). Inconsistent ideas related to definitions and examples. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 12, 31–47.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Esther Levenson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tsamir, P., Tirosh, D. & Levenson, E. Intuitive nonexamples: the case of triangles. Educ Stud Math 69, 81–95 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-008-9133-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-008-9133-5

Keywords

Navigation