Skip to main content
Log in

Community Science, Philosophy of Science, and the Practice of Research

  • Published:
American Journal of Community Psychology

Abstract

Embedded in community science are implicit theories on the nature of reality (ontology), the justification of knowledge claims (epistemology), and how knowledge is constructed (methodology). These implicit theories influence the conceptualization and practice of research, and open up or constrain its possibilities. The purpose of this paper is to make some of these theories explicit, trace their intellectual history, and propose a shift in the way research in the social and behavioral sciences, and community science in particular, is conceptualized and practiced. After describing the influence and decline of logical empiricism, the underlying philosophical framework for science for the past century, I summarize contemporary views in the philosophy of science that are alternatives to logical empiricism. These include contextualism, normative naturalism, and scientific realism, and propose that a modified version of contextualism, known as perspectivism, affords the philosophical framework for an emerging community science. I then discuss the implications of perspectivism for community science in the form of four propositions to guide the practice of research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Albee, G. W. (1996). Revolutions and counterrevolutions in prevention. American Psychologist, 51, 1130–1133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, E. (2003). Feminist epistemology and philosophy of science. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Fall 2003 ed.). Retrieved February 15, 2004 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2003/entries/feminism-epistemology/.

  • Bacon, F. (1620/1972). Novum organum. In D. N. Robinson (Ed.). Readings in the origins and principles of psychology (pp. 43–47). Encino, CA: Dickenson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., & Donnerstein, E. (1982). External validity is more than skin deep: Some answers to criticisms of laboratory experiments. American Psychologist, 37, 245–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhaskar, R. (1978). A realist theory of science. London: Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhaskar, R. (1986). Scientific realism and human emancipation. London, Verso.

  • Biglan, A., Glasgow, R. E., & Singer, G. (1990). The need for a science of larger social units: A contextual approach. Behavior Therapy, 21, 195–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R. (2002). Scientific realism. In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved February 15, 2004 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2002/entries/scientific-realism/>.

  • Brower, B. W. (1998). Contextualism, epistemological. In E. Craig (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy. London: Routledge. Retrieved February 22, 2004 from http://www.rep.routledge.com/article/P058.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, E. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychological Review, 62, 193–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1966). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand-McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1974). Qualitative knowing in action research. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Kurt Lewin Award Address, Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues, New Orleans, LA.

  • Capaldi, E. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1994). Contextualism: Is the act in context the adequate metaphor for scientific psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 239–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capaldi, E. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1999). Contextualism in psychological research: A critical review. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J., & Bronfenbrenner, U. (1991). On the demise of everyday memory: “The rumors of my death are greatly exaggerated” (Mark Twain). American Psychologist, 46, 27–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1990). Things I have learned (so far). American Psychologist, 45, 1304–1312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D. (1985). Postpositivist critical multiplism. In L. Shotland & M. M. Mark (Eds.), Social science and social policy (pp. 21–62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1982). Designing evaluations of educational and social programs. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1987). Social inquiry by and for earthlings. In W. R. Shadish Jr. & C. S. Reichardt (Eds.), Evaluation studies review annual (Vol. 12, pp. 400–424). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Aunno, T., Klein, D. C., & Susskind, E. C. (1985). Seven approaches for the study of community phenomena. In E. C. Susskind & D. C. Klein (Eds.), Community research: Methods, paradigms, and applications (pp. 421–496). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denzin, N. K. (1978). The research act: A theoretical introduction to sociological methods (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C. (2004). Clapping with both hands: Numbers, people, and simultaneous hypotheses. In J. T. Jost, M. R. Banaji, & D. A. Prentice (Eds.), Perspectivism in social psychology: The Yin and Yang of scientific progress (pp. 261–274). Washington: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyerabend, P. (1965). Reply to criticism. In R. S. Cohen, & M. W. Wartofsky (Eds.), Boston studies in the philosophy of science (Vol. II, pp. 223–261). New York: Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyerabend, P. (1975). Against method: Outline of an anarchist theory of knowledge. London: New Left Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A. (1998). Scientific realism and antirealism. In E. Craig (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy. London: Routledge. Retrieved February 22, 2004 from http://www.rep.routledge.com/article/Q094SECT2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, K. (1999). Laudan’s naturalistic axiology. Philosophy of Science, 66, 526–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, M. (1998). Logical positivism. In E. Craig (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy. London: Routledge. Retrieved February 22, 2004, from http://www.rep.rout-ledge.com/article/Q061.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 266–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. (1992). Toward a postmodern psychology. In S. Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and postmodernism (pp. 17–30). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage .

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Swijtink, Z. G., Porter, T. M., Daston, L., Beatty, J., & Krueger, L. (1989). The empire of chance: How probability changed science and everyday life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, D. (1992). The mind’s we: Contextualism in cognitive psychology. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, L. (2001). From research to “best practices” in other settings and populations. American Journal of Health Behavior, 25, 165–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, K., & Rappaport, J. (1995). Researching self-help/mutual aid groups and organizations: Many roads, one journey. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 3, 217–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. (2002). Naturalism. The Internet encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved February 15, 2004 from http://www.iep.utm.edu/n/naturalism.htm.

  • Jaeger, M. E., & Rosnow, R. L. (1988). Contextualism and its implications for psychological inquiry. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 63–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jick, T. D. (1979). Mixing qualitative and quantitative methods: Triangulation in action. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 602–611.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (Eds.). (2004). Perspectivism in Social Psychology: The Yin and Yang of Scientific Progress. Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). The estrangement of social constructionism and experimental social psychology: History of the rift and prospects for reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(3), 168–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellam, S. G., & Langevin, D. J. (2003). A framework for understanding “evidence” in prevention research and programs. Prevention Science, 4, 137–153.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J. G. (1990). A guide to conducting prevention research in the community: First steps. New York: Haworth Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J. G. (2003). Science and community psychology: Social norms for pluralistic inquiry. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31(3/4), 213–217.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemp, S., & Holmwood, J. (2003). Realism, regularity, and social explanation. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 33, 165–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kingry-Westergaard, C., & Kelly, J. G. (1990). A contextualist epistemology for ecological research. In P. Tolan, C. Keys, F. Chertok, & L. Jason (Eds.), Researching community psychology: Issues of theory and methods (pp. 23–31). Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kincaid, H. (1998). Positivism in the social sciences. In E. Craig (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy. London: Routledge. Retrieved February 22, 2004, from http://www.rep.routledge.com/article/R017SECT3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kvale, S. (Ed.). (1992). Psychology and postmodernism. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programs. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91–196). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1978). The methodology of scientific research programs. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lather, P. (1986). Research as praxis. Harvard Educational Review, 56, 257–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lather, P. (1991). Getting smart: Feminist research and pedagogy with/in the postmodern. New York: Routledge, Chapman, and Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lather, P. (1992). Critical frames in educational research: Feminist and post-structural perspectives. Theory into Practice, 31, 87–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laudan, L. (1990). Normative naturalism. Philosophy of Science, 57, 44–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudan, L. (1996). Beyond positivism and relativism: Theory, method, and evidence. Boulder, CO: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as a basic process of development: A developmentalist-contextualist perspective. Developmental Psychology, 27, 27–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., DeStefanis, I., & Apfel, A. (2001). Understanding developmental systems in adolescence: Implications for methodological strategies, data analytic approaches, and training. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16(1), 9–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. (1974). Scientific method and the adversary model: Some preliminary thoughts. American Psychologist, 29, 661–677.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. (1980). Investigative reporting as a research method: An analysis of Bernstein and Woodward’s All the President’s Men. American Psychologist, 35, 626–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, L. H. (2000). Self-help groups. In J. Rappaport, & E. Seidman (Eds.), Handbook of community psychology. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longino, H. (2002). The social dimensions of scientific knowledge. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Summer 2002 ed.). Retreived February 15, 2004 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2002/entries/scientific-knowledge-social/.

  • MacCoun, R. J. (1998). Biases in the interpretation and use of research results. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 259–287.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mancuso, J. C. (2002). Critically assessing competing world views: Contextualism in psychological research? Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 15, 75–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mark, M. M. (1986). Validity typologies and the logic and practice of quasi-experimentation. New Directions in Program Evaluation, 31, 47–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, P. (2003). Critical realism: A way forward for evaluation research in nursing? Journal of Advanced Nursing, 43, 411–420.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1983). A contextualist theory of knowledge: Its implications for innovation and reform in psychological research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–47). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1986). A perspectivist looks at contextualism and the future of behavioral science. In R. L. Rosnow, & M. Georgundi (Eds). Contextualism and understanding in behavioral science (pp. 271–303). New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1989). A perspectivist approach to the strategic planning of programmatic scientific research. In B. Gholson, W. R. Shadish, Jr., R. A. Neimeyer, & A. C. Houts (Eds.), Psychology of science: Contributions to metascience (pp. 214–245). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1997). Creative hypothesis generating in psychology: Some useful heuristics. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 1–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1999). Constructing social psychology: Creative and critical processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (2004). Appendix: Perspectivist worksheets for generating a program of research. In J. T. Jost, M. R. Banaji, & D. A. Prentice (Eds.), Perspectivism in social psychology: The Yin and Yang of scientific progress (pp. 319–332). Washington: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, P. J., & Haggerty, R. J. (Eds.) (1994). Reducing risks for mental disorders. Frontiers for preventive intervention research. National Academy Press: Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, G., Prilleltensky, I., & McGillivary, H. (2001). Value-based partnerships: Toward solidarity with oppressed groups. American Journal of Community Psychology, 29, 649–677.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. J., & Pedhazur, L. S. (1991). Measurement, design, and analysis: An integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepper, S. C. (1942). World hypotheses. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlesz, A., & Lindsay, J. (2003). Methodological triangulation in researching families: Making sense of dissonant data. Internal Journal of Social Research Methodology, 6, 25–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polkinghorne, D. E. (1992). Postmodern epistemology of practice. In S. Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and postmoderism (pp. 146–165). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, J. (2002). ‘Paul Feyerabend,” In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Summer 2002 ed.). Retrieved February 15, 2004 from http://sum2002/entries/feyerabend/.

  • Price, R. H., & Behrens, T. (2003). Working Pasteur’s quadrant: Harnessing science and action for community change. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31(3/4), 219–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prilleltensky, I. (2003). Understanding, resisting, and overcoming oppression: Toward psychopolitical validity. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 195–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2001). Empirical evaluation and justification of methodologies in psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 127(6), 759–772.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, J. (1990). Research methods and the empowerment social agenda. In P. Tolan, C. Keys, F. Chertok, & L. Jason (Eds.), Researching community psychology: Issues of theory and methods (pp. 51–63). Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, J. (2000). The three Rs of action research methodology: Reciprocity, reflexivity, and reflection-on-reality. Educational Action Research, 8, 307–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosnow, R. L., & Georgoudi, M., (1986). (Eds.). Contextualism and understanding in behavioral science. Implications for research and theory. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, P. H., Freeman, H. E., & Lipsey, M. W. (1999). Evaluation: A systematic approach. (6th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sankey, H. (1999). Methodological pluralism, normative naturalism and the realist aim of science. In R. Nola & H. Sankey (Eds.), After Popper, Kuhn, and Feyerabend: Recent issues in theories of scientific method, Australasian studies in history and philosphy of science. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sankey, H. (2001). Scientific realism: An elaboration and a defence. Theoria, 98, 35–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Sankey, H. (2002). Realism, method, and truth. In M. Marsonet (Ed.), The problem of realism (pp. 64–81). Ashgate: Aldershot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sechrest, L., & Sidani, S. (1995). Quantitative and qualitative methods: Is there an alternative? Evaluation and Program Planning, 18, 77–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R. (1995). Philosophy of science and the quantitative-qualitative debates: Thirteen common errors. Evaluation and Program Planning, 18, 63–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shinn, M. (1990). Mixing and matching: Levels of conceptualization, measurement, and statistical analysis in community research. In P. Tolan, C. Keys, F. Chertok, & L. Jason (Eds.), Researching community psychology: Issues of theory and methods (pp. 111–126). Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shinn, M., & Toohey, S. M. (2003). Community contexts of human welfare. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 427–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strupp, H. H., & Hadley, S. W. (1977). A tripartite model of mental health and therapeutic outcome: With special reference to negative effects in psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 32, 187–196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sue, D. W. (1991). A diversity perspective on contextualism. Journal of Counseling and Development, 70, 300–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sue, S. (1999). Science, ethnicity, and bias: Where have we gone wrong? American Psychologist, 54(12), 1070–1077.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Susskind, E. C., & Klein, D. C. (Eds.) (1985). Community research: Methods, paradigms, and applications. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tebes, J. K. (1997). Self-help, prevention, and scientific knowledge. Invited Paper to the Pre-Conference Workshop on Self-Help, Biennial Meeting of the Society for Community Research and Action, Columbia, South Carolina.

  • Tebes, J. K. (2000). External validity and scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 55, 1508–1509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tebes, J. K., Kaufman, J. S., & Connell, C. M. (2003). The evaluation of prevention and health promotion programs. In T. P. Gullotta & M. Bloom (Eds.). Encyclopedia of primary prevention and health promotion (pp. 42–61). New York: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tebes, J. K., & Kraemer, D. T. (1991). Quantitative and qualitative knowing in mutual support research: Some lessons from the recent history of scientific psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 739–756.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tebes, J. K., Snow, D. L., Ayers, T. S., & Arthur, M. W. (1996). Panel accretion and external validity in adolescent substance use research. Evaluation Review, 20, 470–484.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolan, P., Keys, C., Chertok, F., & Jason, L. (Eds.). (1990). Researching community psychology: Issues of theory and methods. Washington, DC: APAS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trickett, E. J. (1996). A future for community psychology: The contexts of diversity and the diversity of contexts. American Journal of Community Psychology, 24, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trickett, E. J., Barone, C., & Buchanan, R. M. (1996). Elaborating developmental contextualism in adolescent research and intervention: Paradigm contributions from community psychology. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 245–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wandersman, A. (2003).Community science: Bridging the gap between science and practice with community-centered models. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 227–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, R. J., Williams, N. C., & Jagers, R. J. (2003). Sociopolitical development. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 309–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, C. (1993). Feminist research: Definitions, methodology, methods and evaluation. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 18, 416–423.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jacob Kraemer Tebes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tebes, J.K. Community Science, Philosophy of Science, and the Practice of Research. Am J Community Psychol 35, 213–230 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-005-3399-x

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-005-3399-x

Keywords

Navigation