Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Individual differences in the frequency of voluntary & involuntary episodic memories, future thoughts, and counterfactual thoughts

  • Research
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Voluntary and involuntary mental time travel can take the form of episodic memory, episodic future thinking, and episodic counterfactual thinking. This study uses an individual-differences approach to understand why people engage in these forms of mental time travel. The individual-differences variables include trait-level personality, boredom proneness, depression, anxiety, stress, emotion regulation, mindfulness, mind-wandering, positive and negative affect, rumination, optimism, thinking styles, and time perspective. Across two studies, our results indicate that individual differences underlie these forms of mental time travel. The most unique, episodic counterfactual thinking, was alone positively correlated with negative emotionality and negatively correlated with optimism. We also observe differences as a function of voluntariness and discuss these findings in relation to the cognitively demanding nature of constructing future and counterfactual thoughts. We discuss the importance of distinguishing voluntary from involuntary thinking and assessing episodic counterfactual thinking in relation to episodic memory and episodic future thinking.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and materials

The author has no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, https://osf.io/73ce5/.

References

  • Addis, D. R. (2018). Are episodic memories special? On the sameness of remembered and imagined event simulation. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand, 48(2–3), 64–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19(1), 33–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009). Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia, 47(11), 2222–2238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arch, J. J., Wilcox, R. R., Ives, L. T., Sroloff, A., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2021). Off-task thinking among adults with and without social anxiety disorder: An ecological momentary assessment study. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 269–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, A. M., Walsh, C. R., & Martin, L. (2013). Fantasy proneness and counterfactual thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(4), 469–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, A. M., Walsh, C. R., Briazu, A., & R. (2020). Looking behind and looking ahead: Personality differences in counterfactual and prefactual thinking. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 40(2), 111–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barzykowski, K., & Niedźwieńska, A. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memories are relatively more often reported during high cognitive load tasks. Acta Psychologica, 182, 119–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Pragmatic prospection: How and why people think about the future. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Hofmann, W., Summerville, A., Reiss, P. T., & Vohs, K. D. (2020). Everyday thoughts in time: Experience sampling studies of mental time travel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(12), 1631–1648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). Thinking about the past and future in daily life: An experience sampling study of individual differences in mental time travel. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 805–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benoit, R. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450–457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D. (2021). Involuntary autobiographical memories and their relation to other forms of spontaneous thoughts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1817), 20190693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 352–372.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., Hoyle, R. H., & Rubin, D. C. (2019). The Autobiographical Recollection Test (ART): A measure of individual differences in autobiographical memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(3), 305–318.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Biderman, N., Bakkour, A., & Shohamy, D. (2020). What are memories for? The hippocampus bridges past experience with future decisions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(7), 542–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blix, I., Kanten, A. B., Birkeland, M. S., & Thoresen, S. (2018). Imagining what could have happened: Types and vividness of counterfactual thoughts and the relationship with post-traumatic stress reactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 515.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Branch, J. G., & Zickar, M. J. (2021). Mental time travel into the episodic future, episodic past, and episodic counterfactual past in everyday life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35(1), 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunette, A. M., & Schacter, D. L. (2021). Cognitive mechanisms of episodic simulation in psychiatric populations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 136, 103778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. M. (2016). Counterfactual thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 135–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carciofo, R., & Jiang, P. (2021). Deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering in Chinese students: Associations with mindfulness, affect, personality, and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 180, 110982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carriere, J. S., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chaieb, L., Hoppe, C., & Fell, J. (2022). Mind wandering and depression: a status report. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, I. A., & Maguire, E. A. (2020). Do questionnaires reflect their purported cognitive functions? Cognition, 195, 104114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2021). Spontaneous and deliberate future thinking: A dual process account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(2), 464–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S. N., Smith, D. M., Ragan, K., Suurmond, R., & Armitage, C. J. (2021). Synthesizing the effects of mental simulation on behavior change: Systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(5), 1514–1537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., Hammerschmidt, T., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2018). Boredom: Under-aroused and restless. Consciousness and Cognition, 61, 24–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 96–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Brigard, F. (2014). Is memory for remembering? Recollection as a form of episodic hypothetical thinking. Synthese, 191(2), 155–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Brigard, F., & Giovanello, K. S. (2012). Influence of outcome valence in the subjective experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1085–1096.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Brigard, F., & Parikh, N. (2019). Episodic counterfactual thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 59–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Brigard, F., Addis, D. R., Ford, J. H., Schacter, D. L., & Giovanello, K. S. (2013a). Remembering what could have happened: Neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking. Neuropsychologia, 51(12), 2401–2414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • De Brigard, F., Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013b). Coming to grips with the past: Effect of repeated simulation on the perceived plausibility of episodic counterfactual thoughts. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1329–1334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Brigard, F., Rodriguez, D. C., & Montañés, P. (2017). Exploring the experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking in younger and older adults: A study of a Colombian sample. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 258–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2019). The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: An exploratory study. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 788–804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2020). Involuntary autobiographical memories and future projections in social anxiety. Memory, 28(4), 516–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deng, Y. Q., Li, S., & Tang, Y. Y. (2014). The relationship between wandering mind, depression and mindfulness. Mindfulness, 5(2), 124–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Espino, O., & Byrne, R. M. (2021). How people keep track of what is real and what is imagined: The epistemic status of counterfactual alternatives to reality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(4), 547.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, X., Gu, R., Liang, F., Broster, L. S., Liu, Y., Zhang, D., & Luo, Y. J. (2015). Depressive states amplify both upward and downward counterfactual thinking. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(2), 93–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallford, D. J., & D’Argembeau, A. (2022). Why we imagine our future: Introducing the Functions of Future Thinking Scale (FoFTS). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 44(2), 376–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallford, D. J., Barry, T. J., Austin, D. W., Raes, F., Takano, K., & Klein, B. (2020). Impairments in episodic future thinking for positive events and anticipatory pleasure in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 536–543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoppen, T. H., Heinz-Fischer, I., & Morina, N. (2020). If only… a systematic review and meta-analysis of social, temporal and counterfactual comparative thinking in PTSD. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1737453.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Isacescu, J., Struk, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 31(8), 1741–1748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M. E., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2017). For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1271–1289.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kvavilashvili, L., & Rummel, J. (2020). On the nature of everyday prospection: A review and theoretical integration of research on mind-wandering, future thinking, and prospective memory. Review of General Psychology, 24(3), 210–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K. (2016). Prospection, well-being and memory. Memory Studies, 9(3), 266–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maillet, D., Beaty, R. E., Jordano, M. L., Touron, D. R., Adnan, A., Silvia, P. J., Kwapil, T. R., Turner, G. R., Spreng, R. N., & Kane, M. J. (2018). Age-related differences in mind-wandering in daily life. Psychology and Aging, 33(4), 643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(3), 244–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martarelli, C. S., Bertrams, A., & Wolff, W. (2021). A personality trait-based network of boredom, spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Assessment, 28(8), 1915–1931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni, G. (2019). Involuntary memories and involuntary future thinking differently tax cognitive resources. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(4), 684–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michaelian, K., Perrin, D., & Sant’Anna, A. (2020). Continuities and discontinuities between imagination and memory: The view from philosophy. The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination, 293, 293–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, M. A., Contractor, A. A., Dranger, P., & Shea, M. T. (2016). Unique relations between counterfactual thinking and DSM–5 PTSD symptom clusters. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 8(3), 293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 560.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nicosia, J., & Balota, D. (2021). Dispositional factors account for age differences in self-reported mind-wandering. Psychology and Aging, 4, 421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, P., & Epstein, S. (2011). An experiential thinking style: Its facets and relations with objective and subjective criterion measures. Journal of Personality, 79(5), 1043–1080.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özbek, M., Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2017). Imagining the personal past: Episodic counterfactuals compared to episodic memories and episodic future projections. Memory & Cognition, 45(3), 375–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Özbek, M., Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2020). Characteristics of personally important episodic memories, counterfactual thoughts, and future projections across age and culture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(5), 1020–1033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozdes, A. (2021). How did I do it then? How will I do it later? A theoretical review of the impact of mental time travel on decision-making processes. New Ideas in Psychology, 62, 100869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1842–1846.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robison, M. K., Gath, K. I., & Unsworth, N. (2017). The neurotic wandering mind: An individual differences investigation of neuroticism, mind-wandering, and executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(4), 649–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robison, M. K., Miller, A. L., & Unsworth, N. (2020). A multi-faceted approach to understanding individual differences in mind-wandering. Cognition, 198, 104078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, A. W., & Robins, S. (2022). Episodic memory, simulated future planning, and their evolution. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00601-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Beaty, R. E., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2019). Depression, anxiety, and stress and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(2), 163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(1), 117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spreng, R. N., & Levine, B. (2006). The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition, 34(8), 1644–1651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S., Cheyne, J. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment, 24(3), 346–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Get real: Effects of repeated simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future experiences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., Ampe, L., Baetens, K., Vandekerckhove, M., & Van Overwalle, F. (2013). Counterfactual thinking: An fMRI study on changing the past for a better future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5), 556–564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(1–2), 115–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vowinckel, J. C., Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E. T., & Webster, J. D. (2017). Flourishing in the now: Initial validation of a present-eudaimonic time perspective scale. Time & Society, 26(2), 203–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, C. A., Israel, E. S., Belleau, E., Appleman, L., Forbes, E. E., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Mind-wandering in adolescents predicts worse affect and is linked to aberrant default mode network–salience network connectivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(3), 377–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, J. Q., Szpunar, K. K., Godovich, S. A., Schacter, D. L., & Hofmann, S. G. (2015). Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 36, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The author did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JGB is the sole author of this manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jared G. Branch.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author has no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 12 kb)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Branch, J.G. Individual differences in the frequency of voluntary & involuntary episodic memories, future thoughts, and counterfactual thoughts. Psychological Research 87, 2171–2182 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01802-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01802-2

Navigation