Skip to main content
Log in

On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Simon effect (prolonged RT when the task-irrelevant stimulus location is incongruent with the response side) has been reported to decrease at longer RTs, which is reflected in negative-going delta functions. This finding has been attributed to gradual dissipation of the response automatically activated by the task-irrelevant location information. The Diffusion Model for Conflict Tasks (DMC, Ulrich, Schröter, Leuthold, & Birngruber, Cognitive Psychology 78:148–174, 2015) formally specifies the time-course of this automatic activation process as a pulse-like function. In contrast to alternative views, DMC is consistent with the notion that this time-course is unaffected by the presentation duration of the target stimulus. Therefore, we expected that delta functions are invariant against changes of stimulus duration. This prediction was verified in two Simon task experiments. Consistent with this general result, DMC’s parameter τ which defines the time-course of the automatic response activation was estimated to not meaningfully differ between short and long durations. We argue that our results are coherent with processing architectures that assume a transient automatic process that is virtually unaffected by stimulus duration.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For stability analysis, we explored whether our main results would remain unchanged when DPs are constructed with a different number of percentiles. Therefore, we conducted the same analysis with 5 percentiles (quintiles) and 19 percentiles (ventiles). Most important and analogous to the analysis reported in the main body of the text, the threefold interaction between stimulus duration, congruency, and percentile was insignificant with 5 and 19 percentiles F(4, 76) = 1.23, p = .293 and F(18, 342) = 0.91, p = .404, respectively, while the twofold interaction of congruency and percentile was significant with 5 bins F(4, 76) = 9.13, p = .001 and also with 19 bins, F(18, 342) = 7.33, p < .001.

  2. Like in Experiment 1, we examined whether our main results would remain unchanged when DPs are constructed with 5 percentiles (quintiles) and 19 percentiles (ventiles). Again, the threefold interaction between stimulus duration, congruency, and percentile was insignificant with 5 and 19 percentiles F(4, 76) = 1.25, p = .297 and F(18, 342) = 0.69, p = .510, respectively, while the twofold interaction of congruency and percentile was significant with both 5 and 19 percentiles, F(4, 76) = 15.55, p < .001, and F(18, 342) = 12.13, p < .001, respectively.

  3. We are well aware of the possibility that the true effect may be too small to be detected by the present experiments due to insufficient statistical power. Power calculations with the function “pwr.f2.test” of the R package “pwr” reveal a power of approximately 87% for detecting a medium effect (i.e., Cohen’s f2 = 0.15) after correcting the degrees of freedom for violation of sphericity (i.e., ε = 0.6, estimated from our results) according to (Geisser & Greenhouse, **1958). Therefore, the probability of detecting a medium effect (i.e., f2 = 0.35; Cohen, 1988) in at least of one of the two experiments would be 1-(1-0.87)2 = 0.98. A similar calculation shows that a sample size of n = 117 per experiment would be required to detect a small effect (f2 = 0.02) with probability 95% in at least of one of the two experiments assuming again ε = 0.6.

References

  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 495–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hilldale. NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coles, M. G. H., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 11, 529–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dambacher, M., & Hübner, R. (2015). Time pressure affects the efficiency of perceptual processing in decisions under conflict. Psychological Research, 79, 83–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 731–750.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellinghaus, R., & Miller, J. (in press). Delta plots with negative-going slopes as a potential marker of decreasing response activation in masked semantic priming. Psychological Research.

  • Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F-distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heathcote, A., Popiel, S., & Mewhort, D. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: An example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1993). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 55, 280–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56, 261–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychologica, 136, 189–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hübner, R., Steinhauser, M., & Lehle, C. (2010). A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. Psychological Review, 117, 759–784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. G. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task. Memory & Cognition, 7, 166–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, C., & Proctor, R. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattler, U. (2003). Delayed flanker effects on lateralized readiness potentials. Experimental Brain Research, 151, 272–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 247–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelder, B. J. A., & Mead, R. (1964). A simplex method for function minimization. The Computer Journal, 7, 308–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action. Consciousness & Self-Regulation, 4, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 242–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & Smith, P. L. (2004). A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. Psychological Review, 111, 333–367.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002a). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action, attention and performance XIX (pp. 494–519). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002b). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 66, 312–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruthruff, E. (1996). A test of the deadline model for speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 56–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 555–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Servant, M., Montagnini, A., & Burle, B. (2014). Conflict tasks and the diffusion framework: Insight in model constraints based on psychological laws. Cognitive Psychology, 72, 162–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Servant, M., White, C., Montagnini, A., & Burle, B. (2016). Liniking theoretical decision-making mechanisms in the Simon task with electrophysiological data: A model-based neuroscience study in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 1501–1521.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Parsing a cognitive task: A characterization of the mind’s bottleneck. Plos Biology, 3, 334–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., Acosta, E., Mewaldt, S. P., & Speidel, C. R. (1976). The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 19, 16–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. L., & Ratcliff, R. (2009). An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection. Psychological Review, 116, 283–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, M. (1960). Models for choice-reaction time. Psychometrika, 25, 251–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Rinkenauer, G., & Miller, J. (1998). Effects of stimulus duration and intensity on simple reaction time and response force. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 915–928.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2016). Corrigendum to Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148–174]. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallesi, A., Mapelli, D., Schiff, S., Amodio, P., & Umiltà, C. (2005). Horizontal and vertical Simon effect: different underlying mechanisms? Cognition, 96, B33–B43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallesi, A., & Umiltà, C. A. (2009). Decay of stimulus spatial code in horizontal and vertical Simon tasks. The Journal of General Psychology, 136, 350–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus-response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 31, 453–464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xiong, A., & Proctor, R. W. (2016). Decreasing auditory Simon effects across reaction time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 23–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by DFG Grant BA 4110/5-2. We thank Birte Forstmann and Ronald Hübner for helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ruben Ellinghaus.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ellinghaus, R., Karlbauer, M., Bausenhart, K.M. et al. On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Psychological Research 82, 734–743 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0860-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0860-z

Navigation