Skip to main content
Log in

Impaired social cognition in violent offenders: perceptual deficit or cognitive bias?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Aggressive behavior is assumed to be associated with certain patterns of social information processing. While some theories link aggression to a tendency to interpret ambiguous stimuli as hostile (i.e., enhanced sensitivity to anger), others assume an insufficient ability to perceive emotional expressions, particularly fear. Despite compelling evidence to support both theories, no previous study has directly investigated the predictions made by these two accounts in aggressive populations. The aim of the current study was to test processing patterns for angry and fearful facial expressions in violent offenders (VOs) and healthy controls (CTLs) and their association with self-reported aggression and psychopathy scores. In Experiment 1, we assessed perceptual sensitivity to neutral-emotional (angry, fearful, happy) blends in a task which did not require categorization, but an indication whether the stimulus is neutral or emotional. In Experiment 2, we assessed categorization performance for ambiguous fearful-happy and angry-happy blends. No group differences were revealed in Experiment 1, while Experiment 2 indicated a deficit in the categorization of ambiguous fearful blends in the VO group. Importantly, this deficit was associated with both self-reported psychopathy and aggression in the VO, but not the CTL group. The current study provides evidence for a deficient categorization of fearful expressions and its association with self-reported aggression and psychopathy in VOs, but no support for heightened sensitivity to anger. Furthermore, the current findings indicate that the deficit is tied to categorization but not detection stages of social information processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Crick NR, Dodge KA (1994) A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychol Bull 115:74–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Crick NR, Dodge KA (1996) Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Dev 67:993–1002

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Oostermeijer S, Nieuwenhuijzen M, van de Ven P, Popma A, Jansen L (2016) Social information processing problems related to reactive and proactive aggression of adolescents in residential treatment. Pers Indiv Differ 90:54–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Dodge KA (2006) Translational science in action: hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol 18:791–814

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Nasby W, Hayden B, DePaulo BM (1980) Attributional bias among aggressive boys to interpret unambiguous social stimuli as displays of hostility. J Abnorm Psychol 89:459–468

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. De Castro BO, Veerman JW, Koops W, Bosch JD, Monshouwer HJ (2002) Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis. Child Dev 73:916–934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Wilkowski BM, Robinson MD (2012) When aggressive individuals see the world more accurately the case of perceptual sensitivity to subtle facial expressions of anger. Pers Soc Psychol B 38:540–553

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Mellentin AI, Dervisevic A, Stenager E, Pilegaard M, Kirk U (2015) Seeing enemies? A systematic review of anger bias in the perception of facial expressions among anger-prone and aggressive populations. Aggress Violent Behav 25:373–383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Burt SA, Mikolajewski AJ, Larson CL (2009) Do aggression and rule-breaking have different interpersonal correlates? A study of antisocial behavior subtypes, negative affect, and hostile perceptions of others. Aggress Behav 35:453–461

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hall CW (2006) Self-reported aggression and the perception of anger in facial expression photos. J Psychol 140:255–267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Knyazev GG, Bocharov AV, Slobodskoj-Plusnin JY (2009) Hostility and gender related differences in oscillatory responses to emotional facial expressions. Aggress Behav 35:502–513

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hoaken PNS, Allaby DB, Earle J (2007) Executive cognitive functioning and the recognition of facial expressions of emotion in incarcerated violent offenders, non-violent offenders, and controls. Aggress Behav 33:412–421

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Schönenberg M, Jusyte A (2014) Investigation of the hostile attribution bias toward ambiguous facial cues in antisocial violent offenders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 264:61–69

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Penton-Voak IS, Thomas J, Gage SH, McMurran M, McDonald S, Munafò MR (2013) Increasing recognition of happiness in ambiguous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive behavior. Psychol Sci 24:688–697

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Blair R (1995) A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57:1–29

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Blair R (2001) Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 71:727–731

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Marsh AA, Blair R (2008) Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:454–465

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Blair R, Mitchell D, Peschardt K, Colledge E, Leonard R, Shine J, Murret L, Perrett D (2004) Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Pers Indiv Differ 37:1111–1122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Eisenbarth H, Alpers GW, Segrè D, Calogero A, Angrilli A (2008) Categorization and evaluation of emotional faces in psychopathic women. Psychiatry Res 159:189–195

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hastings ME, Tangney JP, Stuewig J (2008) Psychopathy and identification of facial expressions of emotion. Pers Indiv Differ 44:1474–1483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Kosson DS, Suchy Y, Mayer AR, Libby J (2002) Facial affect recognition in criminal psychopaths. Emotion 2:398

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Montagne B, van Honk J, Kessels RP, Frigerio E, Burt M, van Zandvoort MJ, de Haan EH (2005) Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Pers Indiv Differ 38:5–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Dolan M, Fullam R (2006) Face affect recognition deficits in personality-disordered offenders: association with psychopathy. Psychol Med 36:1563–1569

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, Lockwood PL, Cecil CA, De Brito SA, McCrory EJ (2012) Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry 169:1109–1116

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. White SF, Williams WC, Brislin SJ, Sinclair S, Blair KS, Fowler KA, Pine D, Pope K, Blair RJ (2012) Reduced activity within the dorsal endogenous orienting of attention network to fearful expressions in youth with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits. Dev Psychopathol 24:1105–1116

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Sato W, Uono S, Matsuura N, Toichi M (2009) Misrecognition of facial expressions in delinquents. Child Adol Psychiatry Ment Health 3:27–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Schönenberg M, Christian S, Gaußer A-K, Mayer SV, Hautzinger M, Jusyte A (2014) Addressing perceptual insensitivity to facial affect in violent offenders: first evidence for the efficacy of a novel implicit training approach. Psychol Med 44:1043–1052

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Schönenberg M, Louis K, Mayer S, Jusyte A (2013) Impaired identification of threat-related social information in male delinquents with antisocial personality disorder. J Pers Disord 27:496

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Schönenberg M, Mayer SV, Christian S, Louis K, Jusyte A (2015) Facial affect recognition in violent and nonviolent antisocial behavior subtypes. J Pers Disord. doi:10.1521/pedi_2015_29_217

  30. Dadds MR, El Masry Y, Wimalaweera S, Guastella AJ (2008) Reduced eye gaze explains “fear blindness” in childhood psychopathic traits. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 47:455–463

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Jusyte A, Mayer SV, Künzel E, Hautzinger M, Schönenberg M (2015) Unemotional traits predict early processing deficit for fearful expressions in young violent offenders: an investigation using continuous flash suppression. Psychol Med 45:285–297

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Sylvers PD, Brennan PA, Lilienfeld SO (2011) Psychopathic traits and preattentive threat processing in children a novel test of the fearlessness hypothesis. Psychol Sci 22:1280–1287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Leist T, Dadds MR (2009) Adolescents’ ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 14:237–250

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Fairchild G, Stobbe Y, Van Goozen SH, Calder AJ, Goodyer IM (2010) Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biol Psychiatry 68:272–279

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Stevens D, Charman T, Blair R (2001) Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Genet Psychol 162:201–211

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Best M, Williams JM, Coccaro EF (2002) Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99:8448–8453

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Schwenck C, Gensthaler A, Romanos M, Freitag CM, Schneider W, Taurines R (2014) Emotion recognition in girls with conduct problems. Eur Child Adoles Psychiatry 23:13–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Buss AH, Perry M (1992) The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol 63:452–459

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Neumann CS, Schmitt DS, Carter R, Embley I, Hare RD (2012) Psychopathic traits in females and males across the globe. Behav Sci Law 30:557–574

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Formann A, Piswanger K (1979) Wiener Matrizen-Test. Manual: Beltz Test Gesellschaft, Weinheim

    Google Scholar 

  41. Formann AK, Waldherr K, Piswanger K (2011) Wiener Matrizen-Test 2. Manual: Beltz Test GmbH, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  42. Langner O, Dotsch R, Bijlstra G, Wigboldus DHJ, Hawk ST, Van Knippenberg A (2010) Presentation and validation of the Radboud faces database. Cogn Emot 24:1377–1388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Dawel A, O’Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R (2012) Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:2288–2304

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Wilson K, Juodis M, Porter S (2011) Fear and loathing in psychopaths: a meta-analytic investigation of the facial affect recognition deficit. Crim Justice Behav 38:659–668

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Short RM, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Adams WJ, Fairchild G (2016) Does comorbid anxiety counteract emotion recognition deficits in conduct disorder? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57:916–926

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Blair R (2007) Empathic dysfunction in psychopathic individuals. In: Farrow TD, Woodruff PWR (eds) Empathy in mental illness. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 3–16

    Google Scholar 

  47. Burt SA (2012) How do we optimally conceptualize the heterogeneity within antisocial behavior? An argument for aggressive versus non-aggressive behavioral dimensions. Clin Psychol Rev 32:263–279

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Frick PJ, White SF (2008) Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:359–375

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Pollak SD (2008) Mechanisms linking early experience and the emergence of emotions illustrations from the study of maltreated children. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17:370–375

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Pollak SD, Kistler DJ (2002) Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expressions of emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99:9072–9076

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Pollak SD, Sinha P (2002) Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev Psychol 38:784–794

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Schönenberg M, Schneidt A, Wiedemann E, Jusyte A (2015) Processing of dynamic affective information in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. doi:10.1177/1087054715577992

  53. Stein T, Peelen MV (2015) Content-specific expectations enhance stimulus detectability by increasing perceptual sensitivity. J Exp Psychol 144:1089–1104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Sevgi M, Diaconescu AO, Tittgemeyer M, Schilbach L (2015) Social Bayes: using Bayesian modeling to study autistic trait-related differences in social cognition. Biol Psychiatry 80:112–119

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Van den Stock J, Righart R, De Gelder B (2007) Body expressions influence recognition of emotions in the face and voice. Emotion 7:487–494

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Lin P-I, Hsieh C-D, Juan C-H, Hossain MM, Erickson CA, Lee Y-H, Su M-C (2016) Predicting aggressive tendencies by visual attention bias associated with hostile emotions. PLoS ONE 11:e0149487

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Blair R (2010) Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: a targeted review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 12:76–82

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Blair R (2007) The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends Cogn Sci 11:387–392

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Schilbach L (2016) Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry. Philos Trans R Soc B 371:20150081

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Sandra Christian and Sarah Mayer for the support in data collection, and Ryan Dutton for language editing.

Funding

This research was funded by the LEAD Graduate School [GSC1028], a project of the Excellence Initiative of the German federal and state governments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aiste Jusyte.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict to declare.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jusyte, A., Schönenberg, M. Impaired social cognition in violent offenders: perceptual deficit or cognitive bias?. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 267, 257–266 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0727-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0727-0

Keywords

Navigation