Skip to main content
Log in

Click-based echolocation in bats: not so primitive after all

  • Review
  • Published:
Journal of Comparative Physiology A Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Echolocating bats of the genus Rousettus produce click sonar signals, using their tongue (lingual echolocation). These signals are often considered rudimentary and are believed to enable only crude performance. However, the main argument supporting this belief, namely the click’s reported long duration, was recently shown to be an artifact. In fact, the sonar clicks of Rousettus bats are extremely short, ~50–100 μs, similar to dolphin vocalizations. Here, we present a comparison between the sonar systems of the ‘model species’ of laryngeal echolocation, the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus), and that of lingual echolocation, the Egyptian fruit bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus). We show experimentally that in tasks, such as accurate landing or detection of medium-sized objects, click-based echolocation enables performance similar to laryngeal echolocators. Further, we describe a sophisticated behavioral strategy for biosonar beam steering in clicking bats. Finally, theoretical analyses of the signal design—focusing on their autocorrelations and wideband ambiguity functions—predict that in some aspects, such as target ranging and Doppler-tolerance, click-based echolocation might outperform laryngeal echolocation. Therefore, we suggest that click-based echolocation in bats should be regarded as a viable echolocation strategy, which is in fact similar to the biosonar used by most echolocating animals, including whales and dolphins.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Abbreviations

FM:

Frequency modulated

CF–FM:

Constant frequency–frequency modulated

ACRF:

Auto-correlation function

WBAF:

Wideband ambiguity function

SPL:

Sound pressure level

SNR:

Signal-to-noise ratio

References

  • Altes RA, Titlebaum EL (1970) Bat signals as optimally Doppler tolerant waveforms. J Acoust Soc Am 48:1014–1020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Au WWL (1993) The Sonar of dolphins. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Beyer RP (1990) A computational model of the cochlea using the immersed boundary method. J Comput Physics 98:145–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boonman AM, Ostwald J (2007) A modeling approach to explain pulse design in bats. Biol Cybern 2:159–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boonman AM, Parsons S, Jones G (2003) The influence of flight speed on the ranging performance of bats using frequency modulated echolocation pulses. J Acoust Soc Am 113:617–628

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahlander DA (1962) Echo location with wide-band waveforms: bat sonar signals. Technical report. MIT Lexington Lincoln Laboratory, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis WE (1952) Quantitative studies of echolocation in bats (Myotis lucifugus); studies of vision of bats (Myotis lucifugus and Eptesicus fuscus) and quantitative studies of vision of owls (Tyto alba pratincola). Ph.D thesis, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York

  • Ghose K, Moss CF (2003) The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. J Acoust Soc Am 114:1120–1131

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghose K, Moss CF (2006) Steering by hearing: a bat’s acoustic gaze is linked to its flight motor output by a delayed, adaptive linear law. J Neurosci 26:1704–1710

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin DR, Novick A, Kornfield M (1958) The sensitivity of echolocation in the fruit bat Rousettus. Biol Bull 155:107–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grinnell AD, Hagiwara S (1972) Studies of auditory neurophysiology in non-echolocating bats, and adaptations for echolocation in one genus, Rousettus. J Comp Physiol A 76:82–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackbarth H (1986) Phase evaluation in hypothetical receivers simulating ranging in bats. Biol Cybernet 54:281–287

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hartley DJ (1992) Stabilization of perceived echo amplitudes in echolocating bats.2. The acoustic behavior of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, when tracking moving prey. J Acoust Soc Am 91:1133–1149

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert HV (1985) Echoortungsverhalten des flughundes Rousettus aegyptiacus (Megachiroptera). Z Saugertierk 50:141–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt MJ, Meddis R (1991) An evaluation of eight computer models of mammalian inner hair-cell function. J Acoust Soc Am 90:904–917

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holderied MW, Korine C, Fenton MB, Parsons S, Robson S, Jones G (2005) Echolocation call intensity in the aerial hawking bat Eptesicus bottae (Vespertilionidae) studied using stereo videogrammetry. J Exp Biol 208:1321–1327

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holderied MW, Baker CJ, Vespe M, Jones G (2008) Understanding signal design during the pursuit of aerial insects by echolocating bats: tools and applications. Integ Comp Biol 48:74–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland RA, Waters DA (2005) Echolocation signals and pinnae movements in the fruit bat Rousettus aegyptiacus. Acta Chiropt 7:83–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland RA, Waters DA, Rayner JM (2004) Echolocation signal structure in the Megachiropteran bat Rousettus aegyptiacus Geoffroy 1810. J Exp Biol 207:4361–4369

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibsen SD, Au WWL, Nachtigall PE, Breese M (2009) Functional bandwidth of an echolocating Atlantic bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). J Acoust Soc Am 125:1214–1221

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson M, Madsen PT, Zimmer WMX, Aguilar de Soto N, Tyack PL (2004) Beaked whales echolocate on prey. Proc Biol Sci 271:383–386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones G, Holderied MW (2007) Bat echolocation calls: adaptation and convergent evolution. Proc Biol Sci 274:905–912

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones G, Teeling EC (2006) The evolution of echolocation in bats. Trends Ecol Evol 21:149–156

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly EJ, Wishner RP (1965) Matched-filter theory for high-velocity accelerating targets. IEEE Trans Mil Electron Mil 9:56–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koay G, Heffner HE, Heffner RS (1997) Audiogram of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus). Hear Res 105:202–210

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koay G, Heffner RS, Heffner HE (1998) Hearing in a Megachiropteran fruit bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus). J Comp Psychol 112:371–382

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi M, Knudsen EI (1979) The oil-bird: hearing and echolocation. Science 204:425–427

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Levanon N, Mozeson E (2004) Radar signals. Wiley, Hoboken

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuo I, Kunugiyama K, Yano M (2004) An echolocation model for range discrimination of multiple closely spaced objects: transformation of spectrogram into the reflected intensity distribution. J Acoust Soc Am 115:920–928

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melcón ML, Denzinger A, Schnitzler H-U (2007) Aerial hawking and landing: approach behaviour in Natterer’s bats, Myotis nattereri (Kuhl 1818). J Exp Biol 210:4457–4464

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melcón ML, Schnitzler H-U, Denzinger A (2009) Variability of the approach phase of landing echolocating greater mouse-eared bats. J Comp Physiol A 195:69–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menne D, Hackbarth H (1986) Accuracy of distance measurement in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: theoretical aspects and computer simulations. J Acoust Soc Am 79:386–397

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Menne D, Kaipf I, Wagner I, Ostwald J, Schnitzler H-U (1989) Range estimation by echolocation in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: trading of phase versus time cues. J Acoust Soc Am 85:2642–2650

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Möhres F, Kulzer H (1956) Ueber die orientierung der flughund (chiroptera-pteropodidae). Z Vergl Physiol 38:1–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morisaka T, Connor RC (2007) Predation by killer whales (Orcinus orca) and the evolution of whistle loss and narrow-band high frequency clicks in odontocetes. J Evolut Biol 20:1439–1458

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moss CF, Surlykke A (2001) Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats. J Acoust Soc Am 110:2207–2226

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moss CF, Bohn K, Gilkenson H, Surlykke A (2006) Active listening for spatial orientation in a complex auditory scene. PLoS Biol 4:e79

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura K, Akamatsu T (2004) Comparison of click characteristics among odontocete species. In: Thomas JA, Moss CF, Vater M (eds) Echolocation in bats and dolphins. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 36–39

    Google Scholar 

  • Peremans H, Hallam J (1998) The spectrogram correlation and transformation receiver, revisited. J Acoust Soc Am 104:1101–1110

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pye JD, Flinn M, Pye A (1962) Correlated orientation sounds and ear movements of horseshoe bats. Nature 196:1186–1188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saillant PA, Simmons JA, Dear SP, McMullen TA (1993) A computational model of echo processing and acoustic imaging in frequency-modulated echolocating bats: the spectrogram correlation and transformation receiver. J Acoust Soc Am 94:2691–2712

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schnitzler H-U, Moss CF, Denzinger A (2003) From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evolut 18:386–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA (1979) Perception of echo phase information in bat sonar. Science 204:1336–1338

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA, Stein RA (1980) Acoustic imaging in bat sonar: echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation. J Comp Physiol 135:61–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA, Fenton MB, O’Farrell MJ (1979) Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. Science 203:16–21

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA, Ferragamo M, Moss CF, Stevenson S, Altes RA (1990) Discrimination of jittered sonar echoes by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus: the shape of target images in echolocation. J Comp Physiol A 167:589–616

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skolnik ML (2001) Introduction to Radar systems. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Speakman JR, Racey PA (1991) No cost of echolocation for bats in flight. Nature 350:421–423

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Springer MS, Stanhope MJ, Madsen O, de Jong WW (2004) Molecules consolidate the placental mammal tree. Trends Ecol Evolut 19:430–438

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Summers CA (1983) Acoustic orientation in the Megachiropteran bat Rousettus. PhD thesis, Indiana University, Bloomington

  • Surlykke A, Moss CF (2000) Echolocation behavior of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, in the field and the laboratory. J Acoust Soc Am 108:2419–2429

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Surlykke A, Boel Pedersen S, Jakobsen L (2009a) Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field. Proc Biol Sci 276:853–860

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surlykke A, Ghose K, Moss CF (2009b) Acoustic scanning of natural scenes by echolocation in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Exp Biol 212:1011–1020

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suthers RA, Summers CA (1980) Behavioral audiogram and masked thresholds of the megachiropteran echolocating bat, Rousettus. J Comp Physiol 136:227–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomassen HA, Djasim UM, David G, Povel D (2004) Echoclick design in swiftlets: single as well as double clicks. Ibis 146:173–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ulanovsky N, Moss CF (2008) What the bat’s voice tells the bat’s brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:8491–8498

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Veselka N, McErlain DD, Holdsworth DW, Eger JL, Chhem RK, Mason MJ, Brain KL, Faure PA, Fenton MB (2010) A bony connection signals laryngeal echolocation in bats. Nature 463:939–942

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Waters DA, Vollrath C (2003) Echolocation performance in the echolocating fruit bat Rousettus aegyptiacus. Acta Chiropt 5:209–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissenbacher P, Wiegrebe L, Kössl M (2002) The effect of preceding sonar emission on temporal integration in the bat, Megaderma lyra. J Comp Physiol A 188:147–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiegrebe L (2008) An autocorrelation model of bat sonar. Biol Cybern 98:587–595

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yovel Y, Falk B, Moss CF, Ulanovsky N (2010) Optimal localization by pointing off axis. Science 327:701–704

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank C. Moss for using her experimental setup to record some of the data reviewed here, B. Falk for help with data acquisition, J. Simmons, H.-U. Schnitzler, T. Akamatsu and K. Beedholm for discussions, R. Holland for helpful comments on the manuscript, and T. Oram for contributing to early stages of the work. This work was funded by a Human Frontiers Science Project (HFSP) grant to N.U., a Weizmann Institute Postdoctoral Fellowship to Y.Y., and a predoctoral fellowship from the Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation to M.G.-S.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nachum Ulanovsky.

Additional information

Y. Yovel and M. Geva-Sagiv contributed equally to this work.

Appendix

Appendix

Appendix A: Energy Flux Density

The term “Energy Flux Density”, which is useful for measuring the energy content of a transient sound wave, was defined in order to allow comparisons of sonar signal intensities between terrestrial and aquatic environments (Au 1993). To do so, the classical Energy Flux definition is modified to take into account both the signal duration and the acoustic impedance of the medium:

$$ E = {\frac{1}{\rho c}}\int\limits_{0}^{T} {P^{2} (t)} dt $$

where E is the energy flux density (in units of J/m2), P(t) is the time-varying sound pressure measured at 1-m from the source, T is the signal duration, ρ is the density of the medium (in units of kg/m3) and c is the sound velocity in the medium.

Appendix B: Wide band ambiguity function

The Ambiguity Function is a common tool in radar and sonar signal-design, used for quantitative estimations of the system’s sensitivity to Doppler shifts, and the effect of Doppler on range measurements. The wide-band ambiguity function (WBAF) considers the actual time-compression or expansion of the signal modeled by the Doppler effect, rather than the frequency-shift approximation which can be used for narrow-band signals (which is sufficient for most radar applications) (Levanon and Mozeson 2004).

The first modifications of the conventional ambiguity-function from radar theory to the case of wideband signals were done by Kelly and Wishner (1965) and Cahlander (1962)—the latter study applied the new wideband theory to analyzing bat signal design. Later, WBAF analysis of bat signals was used by several other researchers (Altes and Titlebaum 1970; Simmons and Stein 1980; Holderied et al. 2008)—all of them used the WBAF to analyze FM signals of laryngeal echolocators. Here, we applied for the first time the WBAF analysis to the case of click-based biosonar, and compared the results to a classic FM call, that of the big brown bat.

The WBAF describes the response of a matched filter to a pure returning echo (with no added noise or attenuation) for a set of target distances and relative speeds: See Fig. 2d. Each horizontal slice through the WBAF is a cross-correlation function calculated between the transmitted signal and a Doppler-distorted version, for a particular relative speed. For our calculations, we used the following version of the WBAF (Kelly and Wishner 1965):

$$ \chi (\tau ,\eta) = \eta^{{\frac{1}{2}}} \int {z(t)z^{*} (\eta (t - \tau ))dt} $$

where z(t) is the bat’s call waveform at time t, * stands for complex conjugation, τ is the time delay to the target (which is proportional to the target’s distance), and η is the Doppler scale factor:

$$ \eta = {\frac{1 + v/c}{1 - v/c}} $$

where v is the relative flight speed of the bat compared to the target, and c is the speed of sound (we used c = 343 m/s for the speed of sound in air).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Yovel, Y., Geva-Sagiv, M. & Ulanovsky, N. Click-based echolocation in bats: not so primitive after all. J Comp Physiol A 197, 515–530 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-011-0639-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-011-0639-4

Keywords

Navigation