Skip to main content
Log in

Stimulus-driven saccades are characterized by an invariant undershooting bias: no evidence for a range effect

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Saccade endpoints are most frequently characterized by an undershooting bias. Notably, however, some evidence suggests that saccades can be made to systematically under- or overshoot a target based on the magnitude of the eccentricities within a given block of trials (i.e., the oculomotor range effect hypothesis). To address that issue, participants completed stimulus-driven saccades in separate blocks of trials (i.e., proximal vs. distal) that entailed an equal number of targets but differed with respect to the magnitude of their eccentricities. In the proximal block, target eccentricities were 3.0°, 5.5°, 8.0°, 10.5° and 13.0°, whereas in the distal block target eccentricities were 10.5°, 13.0°, 15.5°, 18.0° and 20.5°. If the range effect represents a tenable hypothesis, then the magnitude of target eccentricities within each block should selectively influence saccade endpoint bias. More specifically, the eccentricities common to the proximal and distal blocks (i.e., 10.5° and 13.0°) should elicit a systematic under- and overshooting bias, respectively. Results for the proximal and distal blocks showed a reliable undershooting bias across target eccentricities, and a direct comparison of the common eccentricities indicated that the undershooting bias was not modulated between blocks. Moreover, our results show that the presence of online target vision did not influence the undershooting bias. Thus, the present findings provide no support for an oculomotor range effect; rather, results evince the mediation of saccades via a control strategy that minimizes movement time and/or the energy requirements of the response.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A Web of Science search indicated that Kapoula (1985) and Kapoula and Robinson (1986) have been cited 84 and 72 times, respectively. A Google Scholar search indicated 107 and 97 citations for each article.

  2. We define the saccades studied here as being stimulus-driven given that the onset of an exogenously presented visual target served as the response imperative. Notably, stimulus-driven saccades involve direct spatial mapping between stimulus and response, and their exogenous presentation entails minimal top-down influence (Rossetti et al. 2005).

  3. Saccade amplitudes in the left and right visual fields did not reliably differ (F < 1). For that reason, visual field was a collapsed factor in our ANOVA model.

  4. Kapoula (1985) interpreted her data via descriptive statistics (i.e., means) and did not include inferential analyses. However, our inferential analyses of the participant-specific data associated with Kapoula’s proximal block (see Table 1 of that work) via single-sample t statistics contrasting saccade amplitudes to veridical target location yielded the following 2.7°: t(3) = 22.26, p < 0.001; 4.4°: t(3) = 0.65, p = 0.56; 6.1°: t(3) = −0.78, p = 0.48; 7.8°: t(3) = −2.86, p = 0.06; 9.5°: t(3) = −3.35, p < 0.05. Thus, only the most proximal and most distal target eccentricities produced amplitudes that reliably differed from veridical target location. Kapoula’s distal block included two participants; we therefore did not submit those data to inferential examination.

References

  • Abrams RA, Meyer DE, Kornblum SK (1989) Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system. J Exp Psychol 15:529–543

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Becker W (1989) Metrics. In: Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (eds) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 13–67

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker W, Fuchs AF (1969) Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Visi Res 9:1247–1258

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Binsted G, Elliott D (1999) Ocular perturbations and retinal/extraretinal information: the coordination of saccadic and manual movements. Exp Brain Res 127:193–206

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dafoe JM, Armstrong IT, Munoz DP (2007) The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Exp Brain Res 179:563–570

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel H, Wolf W, Hauske G (1986) Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. Hum Neurobiol 5:245–253

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ehresman C, Saucier D, Heath M, Binsted G (2008) Online corrections can produce illusory bias during closed-loop pointing. Exp Brain Res 188:371–378

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Hansen S, Mendoza J, Tremblay L (2004) Learning to optimize speed, accuracy and energy expenditure: a framework for understanding speed-accuracy relations in goal-directed aiming. J Mot Behav 36:339–351

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engelbrecht SE, Berthier NE, O’Sullivan LP (2003) The undershoot bias: learning to act optimally under uncertainty. Psychol Sci 14:257–261

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evdokimidis I, Tsekou H, Smyrnis N (2006) The mirror antisaccade task: direction-amplitude interaction and spatial accuracy characteristics. Exp Brain Res 174:304–311

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM, Peterson JR (1964) Information capacity of discrete motor responses. J Exp Psychol 67:103–112

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gaveau V, Martin O, Prablanc C, Pélisson D, Urquizar C, Desmurget M (2003) On-line modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals. NeuroReport 14:875–878

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerardin P, Gaveau V, Pèlisson D, Prablanc C (2011) Integration of visual information for saccade production. Hum Mov Sci 30:1009–1021

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA (2011) Transforming vision into action. Vis Res 51:1567–1587

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallett PE (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vis Res 18:1279–1296

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harris CM (1995) Does saccadic undershoot minimize saccadic flight-time? A Monte-Carlo study. Vis Res 35:691–701

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harris CM, Jacobs M, Shawkat F, Taylor D (1993) The development of saccadic accuracy in the first seven months. Clin Vision Sci 8:85–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Dunham K, Binsted G, Godbolt B (2010) Antisaccades exhibit diminished online control relative to prosaccades. Exp Brain Res 203:743–752

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Weiler J, Marriott K, Welsh TN (2011) Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades. Exp Brain Res 209:117–127

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kapoula Z (1985) Evidence for a range effect in the saccadic system. Vis Res 25:1155–1157

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kapoula Z, Robinson DA (1986) Saccadic undershoot is not inevitable: saccades can be accurate. Vis Res 26:735–743

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Khan MA, Elliot D, Coull J, Chua R, Lyons J (2002) Optimal control strategies under different feedback schedules: kinematic evidence. J Mot Behav 34:45–57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knox PC, Bruno N (2007) When does action resist visual illusion? The effect of Müller–Lyer stimuli on reflexive and voluntary saccades. Exp Brain Res 181:277–287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kowler E, Blaser E (1995) The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets. Vis Res 35:1741–1754

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krappmann P, Everling S, Flohr H (1998) Accuracy of visually and memory-guided antisaccades in man. Vis Res 38:2979–2985

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveira FTP, Elliott D, Goodman D (2005) Energy-minimization bias: compensating for intrinsic influence of energy-minimization mechanisms. Mot Control 9:101–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Jeannerod M (1975) Corrective saccades: dependence on retinal reafferent signals. Vis Res 15:465–469

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson DA (1973) Models of the saccadic eye movement control system. Kybernetik 14:71–83

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti Y, Revol P, McIntosh R, Pisella L, Rode G, Danckert J, Tilikete C, Dijkerman HC, Boisson D, Vighetto A, Michel F, Milner AD (2005) Visually guided reaching: bilateral posterior parietal lesions cause a switch from fast visuomotor to slow cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 43:162–177

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Snedecor GW, Cochran WG (1980) Statistical methods. Iowa State University Press, Ames, IA

    Google Scholar 

  • Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5:1226–1235

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vitu F (1991) Against the existence of a range effect during reading. Vis Res 31:2009–2015

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weber RB, Daroff RB (1971) The metrics of horizontal saccadic eye movements in normal humans. Vis Res 11:921–928

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weiler J, Heath M (2012a) Task-switching in oculomotor control: unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades. Neurosci Lett 530:150–154

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weiler J, Heath M (2012b) The prior-antisaccade effect influences the planning and online control of prosaccades. Exp Brain Res 216:545–552

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiler J, Holmes SA, Mulla A, Heath M (2011) Pro- and antisaccades: dissociating stimulus and response influences the online control of saccade trajectories. J Mot Behav 43:375–381

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West GL, Welsh TN, Pratt J (2009) Saccadic trajectories receive online correction: evidence for a feedback-based system of oculomotor control. J Mot Behav 41:117–127

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, and by an Academic Development Fund and Faculty Scholar Award from the University of Western Ontario.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Heath.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gillen, C., Weiler, J. & Heath, M. Stimulus-driven saccades are characterized by an invariant undershooting bias: no evidence for a range effect. Exp Brain Res 230, 165–174 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3640-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3640-z

Keywords

Navigation