Skip to main content
Log in

Seeing the light: exploring the Colavita visual dominance effect

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Colavita visual dominance effect refers to the phenomenon whereby participants presented with unimodal auditory, unimodal visual, or bimodal audiovisual stimuli in a speeded discrimination task, fail to respond to the auditory component of bimodal targets significantly more often than they fail to respond to the visual component. The Colavita effect was demonstrated in this study when participants were presented with unimodal auditory, unimodal visual, or bimodal stimuli (in the ratios 40:40:20, Experiment 1; or 33:33:33, Experiment 2), to which they had to respond by pressing an auditory response key, a visual response key, or both response keys. The Colavita effect was also demonstrated when participants had to respond to the bimodal targets using a dedicated third (bimodal) response key (Experiment 3). These results therefore suggest that stimulus probability and the response demands of the task do not contribute significantly to the Colavita effect. In Experiment 4, we investigated what role exogenous attention toward a sensory modality plays in the Colavita effect. A significantly larger Colavita effect was observed when a visual cue preceded the bimodal target than when an auditory cue preceded it. This result suggests that the Colavita visual dominance effect can be partially explained in terms of the greater exogenous attention-capturing qualities of visual versus auditory stimuli.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Colavita and his colleagues (Colavita 1974; Colavita et al. 1976; Colavita and Weisberg 1979) defined the ‘prepotency of the visual over the auditory stimulus’ in terms of the fact that participants made more light-key than tone-key responses on the bimodal trials. In contrast, Egeth and Sager (1977) defined visual dominance in terms of the slowing of reaction times (RTs) to an auditory stimulus which they attributed to the presence of a concurrent visual stimulus. Subsequent research has, however, most frequently reverted to Colavita’s original definition (e.g., Koppen and Spence in press, submitted a, b; Sinnett et al. in press).

  2. It should be noted that the task that participants had to perform could also be considered to be a modality detection task. Quite what is the most appropriate description for the task is ambiguous, as participants were both detecting stimuli in different modalities, and discriminating which modality a target had been presented in.

  3. It is interesting to note that participants in our experiments responded more rapidly to unimodal than to bimodal targets, which is the reverse pattern of response latencies observed in redundant target paradigms (Miller 1982). The main difference between the Colavita effect and redundant target paradigm lies in the response requirements of the tasks: in the former, participants make modality discriminations, whereas in the latter they tend to make simple detection responses instead.

  4. We re-ran the analysis on the arcsine-transformed error data to ensure normality and homogeneity of variance. This analysis produced the same results. There was a significant main effect of Target Stimulus (Auditory, Bimodal, or Visual) [F(1.09, 14.20) = 21.16, P < 0.001], due to participants responding less accurately on bimodal target trials (P = 0.39; where P is the arcsine-transformed proportion) than on either unimodal auditory (P = 0.19; t(13) = 5.60, P < 0.001) or unimodal visual target trials (P = 0.19; t(13) = 4.14, P = 0.001), but no less accurately on unimodal visual than on unimodal auditory target trials (t(13) = 0.67, P = 0.515).

  5. Note that we did not attempt to match the intensity of the auditory and visual stimuli in the present study (cf. Spence et al. 2001b), which means that the visual stimuli used in our studies might have been subjectively less intense or alerting than the auditory stimuli. However, in this regard it should be noted that Colavita (1974, Experiment 2) attempted to make the auditory stimuli twice as intense as the visual stimulus, but found that the magnitude of the Colavita effect was unaffected by the experimental manipulation of relative subjective target intensity.

References

  • Bertelson P (1961) Sequential redundancy and speed in a serial two-choice responding task. Q J Exp Psychol 13:90–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson P, Aschersleben G (1998) Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location. Psychon Bull Rev 5:482–489

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson P, de Gelder B (2004) The psychology of multimodal perception. In: Spence C, Driver J (eds) Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 141–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Boulter LR (1977) Attention and reaction times to signals of uncertain modality. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform 3:379–388

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell KC, Proctor RW (1993) Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 19:1345–1362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colavita FB (1974) Human sensory dominance. Percept Psychophys 16:409–412

    Google Scholar 

  • Colavita FB, Tomko R, Weisberg D (1976) Visual prepotency and eye orientation. Bull Psychon Soc 8:25–26

    Google Scholar 

  • Colavita FB, Weisberg D (1979) A further investigation of visual dominance. Percept Psychophys 25:345–347

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Driver J, Spence C (2000) Multisensory perception: beyond modularity and convergence. Curr Biol 10:R731–R735

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth HE, Sager LC (1977) On the locus of visual dominance. Percept Psychophys 22:77–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429–433

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot C, Pashler H (1992) Making two responses to a single object: implications for the central attentional bottleneck. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:1058–1079

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Giard MH, Peronnet F (1999) Auditory-visual integration during multimodal object recognition in humans: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. J Cog Neurosci 11:473–490

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin AJ (1895) On the least observable interval between stimuli addressed to disparate senses and to different organs of the same sense. Am J Psychol 6:564–575

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey N (1980) Non-informative effects of stimuli functioning as cues. Q J Exp Psychol 32:413–425

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hay JC, Pick HLJ, Ikeda K (1965) Visual capture produced by prism spectacles. Psychonom Sci 2:215–216

    Google Scholar 

  • Heron J, Whitaker D, McGraw PV (2004) Sensory uncertainty governs the extent of audio-visual interaction. Vis Res 44:2875–2884

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holender D (1980) Interference between a vocal and a manual response to the same stimulus. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behaviour. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 421–431

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard IP, Templeton WB (1966) Human spatial orientation. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson TL, Shapiro KL (1989) Attention to auditory and peripheral visual stimuli: effects of arousal and predictability. Acta Psychol 72:233–245

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein RM (1977). Attention and visual dominance: a chronometric analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 3:365–378

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koppen C, Spence C (in press) Spatial coincidence modulates the Colavita visual dominance effect. Neurosci Lett

  • Koppen C, Spence C (submitted a) Assessing the endogenous attentional account of the Colavita visual dominance effect. Manuscript submitted for publication

  • Koppen C, Spence C (submitted b) Semantic congruency and the Colavita effect. Manuscript submitted for publication

  • Kornblum S (1973) Sequential effects in choice reaction time: a tutorial review. In: Kornblum S (ed) Attention and performance, vol 4. Academic Press, New York, pp 259–288

  • Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Maldjian JA, Burdette JH, Wallace MT (2004) Semantic congruence is a critical factor in multisensory behavioral performance. Exp Brain Res 158:405–414

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGurk H, MacDonald J (1976) Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264:746–748

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller JO (1982) Divided attention: evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cogn Psychol 14:247–279

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molholm S, Ritter W, Murray MM, Javitt DC, Schroeder CE, Foxe JJ (2002) Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. Cogn Brain Res 14:121–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore JJ, Massaro DW (1973) Attention and processing capacity in auditory recognition. J Exp Psychol 99:49–54

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morein-Zamir S, Soto-Faraco S, Kingstone A (2003) Auditory capture of vision: Examining temporal ventriloquism. Cogn Brain Res 17:154–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson R (1973) Intersensory facilitation of reaction time: Energy summation or preparation enhancement? Psychol Rev 80:489–509

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Partan S, Marler P (1999) Communication goes multimodal. Science 283:1272–1273

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler H (1998) The psychology of attention. MIT, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler H, Baylis G (1991) Procedural learning: 2. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 17:33–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI (1978) Chronometric explorations of mind. Erlbaum, Hillsdale

  • Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Klein RM (1976) Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychol Rev 83:157–171

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Ogden WC (1978) Attended and unattended processing modes: the role of set for spatial location. In: Pick HI, Saltzman IJ (eds) Modes of perceiving and processing information. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 137–157

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan P (2000) The ‘late’ locus of visual dominance. Abstr Psychon Soc 5:64

    Google Scholar 

  • Randich A, Klein RM, LoLordo VM (1978) Visual dominance in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav 30:129–137

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Recanzone GH (2003) Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception. J Neurophysiol 89:1078–1093

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rock I, Harris CS (1967, 17 May). Vision and touch. Sci Am 216:96–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock I, Victor J (1964) Vision and touch: an experimentally created conflict between the two senses. Science 143:594–596

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rodway P (2005) The modality shift effect and the effectiveness of warning signals in different modalities. Acta Psychol 120:199–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders AF (1975) The foreperiod effect revisited. Q J Exp Psychol 27:591–598

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A (2002a) E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh Psychology Software Tools Inc.

  • Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A (2002b) E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh Psychology Software Tools Inc.

  • Shams L, Kamitani Y, Shimojo S (2000) What you see is what you hear: Sound induced visual flashing. Nature 408:788

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro KL, Egerman B, Klein RM (1984) Effects of arousal on human visual dominance. Percept Psychophys 35:547–552

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro KL, Jacobs WJ, LoLordo VM (1980) Stimulus-reinforcer interactions in Pavlovian conditioning of pigeons: implications for selective associations. Anim Learn Behav 8:586–594

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimojo S, Shams L (2001) Sensory modalities are not separate modalities: plasticity and interactions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:505–509

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sinnett S, Spence C, Soto-Faraco S (in press) Visual dominance and attention: the Colavita effect revisited. Percept Psychophys

  • Smith WF (1933) The relative quickness of visual and auditory perception. J Exp Psychol 16:239–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soetens E (1998) Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform 10:581–598

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soetens E, Melis A, Notebaert W (2002) Sequence learning and sequential effects. Psychol Res 69:124–137

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Driver J (1997) Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting. Percept Psychophys 59:1–22

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Nicholls MER, Driver J (2001a) The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Percept Psychophys 63:330–336

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Shore DI, Klein RM (2001b) Multisensory prior entry. J Exp Psychol Gen 130:799–832

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener EB (1908) Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. Macmillan, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Turatto M, Benso F, Galfano G, Gamberini L, Umiltà C (2002) Non-spatial attentional shifts between audition and vision. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28:628–639

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turatto M, Galfano G, Bridgeman B, Umiltà C (2004) Space-independent modality-driven attentional capture in auditory, tactile and visual systems. Exp Brain Res 155:301–310

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe K, Shimojo S (1998) Attentional modulation in perception of visual motion events. Perception 27:1041–1054

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB, DuttonHurt LD, Warren DH (1986) Contributions of audition and vision to temporal rate perception. Percept Psychophys 39:294–300

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB, Warren DH (1986) Intersensory interactions. In Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP (eds) Handbook of perception and performance: vol 1. Sensory processes and perception, Wiley, New York, pp 25-1–25-36

  • Zampini M, Shore DI, Spence C (2005) Audiovisual prior entry. Neurosci Lett 381:217–222

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

C. K. was supported by a Departmental Studentship from the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Camille Koppen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koppen, C., Spence, C. Seeing the light: exploring the Colavita visual dominance effect. Exp Brain Res 180, 737–754 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0894-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0894-3

Keywords

Navigation