Cannabis is the most frequently used illicit drug in the Western world, with estimates going up to 18% of Europeans having tried cannabis at least once in their life (Rigter and van Laar 2002). The widespread use of cannabis and its potential for increased therapeutic use have given rise to significant public health concern, mostly regarding the effects of cannabis on cognition and psychomotor function.

The acute effects of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the major psychotropic compound in cannabis, on cognitive and psychomotor performance have been assessed in numerous experimental studies using within-subject, double-blind, placebo-controlled designs. These have generally shown that THC in doses between 40 and 300 μg/kg causes a dose-dependent reduction in performance at tests measuring memory, attention, reaction time (RT), tracking, and motor function (Curran et al. 2002; Hall and Solowij 1998; Heishman et al. 1997). This performance impairment after acute THC administration was shown to be highest during the first hour after smoking or between 1 and 2 h after oral intake (Curran et al. 2002). The number of significant performance effects sharply declined to about zero over 3–4 h after THC use.

It has been demonstrated that frequency and history of cannabis use can affect the performance outcome. In a study by Pope and Yurgelun-Todd (1996), heavy cannabis users (smoking approximately daily) showed significantly greater impairment than light users (smoking on average once a month) on attentional and executive functions after a minimum of 19 h abstinence. In another study by Pope et al. (2001), current heavy users (smoked >5,000 times and smoking daily) demonstrated cognitive impairment up to 7 days after last cannabis use. This impairment was related to levels of carboxy-THC in urine and was no longer present after 28 days of abstinence. Bolla et al. (2002) did demonstrate impairment after 28 days of abstinence in very heavy cannabis users (smoking approximately 35 joints/week), but found no relation with duration of use. In a longitudinal study, Fried et al. (2002) compared IQ scores of light (<5 joints/week) and heavy (≥5 joints/week) current users, former users, and nonusers. These IQ scores were compared with the subjects’ IQ scores that were obtained before initiation of drug use. Fried et al. (2002) found that current marijuana users showed a lower overall IQ, but only in the heavy users group. The former heavy users, who had not used in at least 3 months, showed no decline in IQ score. Therefore, it can be concluded that impairment is apparent in current regular users, even when they are not in an intoxicated state, but that this impairment is not permanent if they stop using (Fried et al. 2002).

From these studies, it can be concluded that cognitive impairments can be demonstrated in cannabis users up to a couple of days after their last use and that this impairment is related to the duration and frequency of use. However, none of these studies have looked at the effect of frequency or duration of use on the acute effects of cannabis.

As with other drugs, regular cannabis use induces tolerance for the acute effects. Previous studies have demonstrated that frequent use of low doses of cannabis can rapidly induce tolerance for the subjective high of cannabis (Cappell and Pliner 1974; Jones et al. 1981). Hart et al. (2001) also demonstrated that heavy cannabis users showed only minimal impairment on complex cognitive tasks after acute cannabis administration. However, it is hard to draw conclusions from this study, as an infrequent cannabis user group was not included. Furthermore, the lack of impairment may be due to the fact that subjects were still impaired in the placebo condition as a result of residual effects. In addition, the exact dose of administered THC was not reported, nor were plasma THC concentrations provided (Nordstrom and Hart 2006; Ramaekers et al. 2006).

We recently investigated neurocognitive performance in heavy (using cannabis >4 times/week) and occasional cannabis users (using cannabis <2 times/week) during acute intoxication (Ramaekers et al. 2009). In this placebo-controlled study, a high dose of THC (500 μg/kg) was administered, while performance measures were conducted four times between 0 and 8 h after smoking. This study demonstrated that the acute effect of THC on human performance is dependent on cannabis use history. Occasional users showed impairments in all performance tasks (i.e., tracking, divided attention task [DAT], and stop signal task [SST]) after smoking cannabis. Heavy users only demonstrated impairment in one performance parameter (i.e., impulse control as measured in the SST) and only at THC concentration above 10 ng/ml in serum. This suggests that heavy cannabis users develop tolerance for some of the impairing effects of cannabis. While this may be the result of pharmacological adaptations, it may also be due to behavioral compensatory mechanisms. Therefore, the behavioral performance of subjects might not be a good indicator for the effect of drugs on brain functioning. Previous studies, using positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging, have demonstrated differences in brain activity in heavy current users and 25-day abstinent heavy users, despite a lack of performance differences (Eldreth et al. 2004; Kanayama et al. 2004). These studies suggest that experienced abstinent users draw on alternative neural networks in order to compensate for the impairment caused by THC.

Event-related potentials (ERPs) are well suited to investigate brain functioning directly. Few studies have examined the effects of cannabis on ERP measures. A consistent finding in those ERP studies is that P300 amplitude is decreased in acutely intoxicated cannabis users (Böcker et al. 2010; Ilan et al. 2004, 2005; Roser et al. 2008) and in cannabis users who are not under the influence of the drug at that time (Kempel et al. 2003; Solowij et al. 1991). The P300 component has previously been shown to reflect attentional resource allocation and working memory (Kok 2001; Polich and Kok 1995). ERPs after acute THC administration are even scarcer. In a single case study, Solowij et al. (1995) showed that acute THC administration after 6 weeks of abstinence normalized the ERP (processing negativity) of a heavy cannabis user during selective attention.

The aim of the present study is to assess ERP measures during THC intoxication in occasional and heavy cannabis users. This will elucidate whether tolerance to the impairing effects of THC can be demonstrated at the neurophysiological level, as has been shown at the behavioral level in heavy cannabis users. This study was part of a larger study of which the results are already published (Ramaekers et al. 2009) and involves the same subjects.

Methods

Subjects

All participants were used to smoking cannabis in combination with tobacco. Twelve occasional cannabis users [8 males and 4 females; mean age 22.8 years (±2.3)] and 12 heavy users [9 males and 3 females; mean age 23.2 years (±3.3)] were included in this study. A summary of the participants’ demographics and drug use characteristic is given in Table 1. Independent-samples t tests showed that there were no differences between groups, except of course on cannabis use per year.

Table 1 Subject demographics (mean, SD) and history of drug use in heavy and occasional cannabis users

Subjects were recruited through advertisements in coffee shops and at the university site. A first screening consisted of a medical and drug questionnaire. Heavy users were selected when smoking more than four times a week, while the occasional group consisted of users that smoked less than twice a week. Selected subjects were subsequently examined by a medical supervisor who checked vital signs and took blood and urine samples. Standard blood chemistry, hematology, and drug screen tests were performed on these samples. Inclusion criteria were free from psychotropic medication; good physical health as determined by the medical examination and laboratory analysis; absence of any major medical, endocrine, and neurological condition; body mass index between 18 and 28 kg/m2; and written informed consent. Exclusion criteria were any history of drug abuse or addiction (other than cannabis); pregnancy or lactation or failure to use reliable contraceptive; excessive drinking (>20 alcoholic consumptions a week); excessive smoking (>25 cigarettes/day); hypertension (diastolic >100; systolic >170); presence or history of a psychiatric illness; color blindness; or dyslexia.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Maastricht University Hospital and conducted according to the World Medical Associations Declaration of Helsinki (Edinburgh modification 2000). A permit for obtaining, storing, and administering marijuana was obtained from the Dutch drug enforcement administration. All subjects received financial compensation for their participation.

Design, doses, and administration

The study was conducted according to a double-blind, placebo-controlled, two-way crossover design. Subjects received placebo and 500 μg/kg body weight THC on two separate test days. Subjects were told that they would receive different doses of THC on separate test days. There was a minimum period of 7 days between treatments. Smoking occurred in the morning of test days. The cigarettes were prepared beforehand for each individual. Cannabis stock was provided by the Dutch Bureau for Medicinal Cannabis and contained 13% THC. The total weight of the cigarette was 0.8 g, containing tobacco and 500 μg/kg body weight THC. This lead to an average cannabis content of 0.256 g for the heavy users group and 0.277 g for the occasional users group. Placebo cigarettes contained 0.8 g of tobacco. Participants were instructed to smoke the cigarette according to a fixed procedure in order to minimize the subject’s possibility of dose titration and to increase optimal absorption of THC (Robbe 1994), i.e., inhale for 4 s, hold breath for 10 s, and exhale/brake for 15 s. This was repeated until the cigarettes were smoked as completely as possible and took 10 to 15 min. Mean (SD) number of puffs smoked from the placebo and THC cigarette was 25 (6.4) and 28 (15.4) for the occasional users and 22 (7.5) and 22 (8.2) for the heavy users, respectively.

Procedures

All participants were asked to refrain from any drug (except cannabis) use during the entire study. The occasional users were asked not to use cannabis for a minimum of 5 days prior to each test day, while the heavy users were to continue using cannabis according to their normal routine. Alcohol was not allowed 24 h prior to each test day and caffeine was not allowed on a test day. Participants were asked to arrive at the site well rested. Drug screens (Mahsan Kombi DOA6+ urine drug test) and alcohol breath analyses were performed upon arrival of the subjects, screening for the presence of alcohol, THC, opiates, amphetamine, cocaine, methamphetamine, and benzodiazepines. For occasional users, administration of the placebo or THC cigarette was only continued if drug and alcohol tests were negative. In case of a positive drug test, subjects were sent home and their test day was rescheduled. For the heavy users, administration of the cigarette (THC or placebo) was only continued if they were positive for THC, but negative for other drugs and alcohol.

Prior to the first test session, subjects received training in order to familiarize them with the tests and procedures and minimize practice effects. On test days, subjects received a standard breakfast prior to smoking. The performance tasks and ERP registration began 20 min after smoking.

ERP measures during task performance

An electro-cap with 30 tin electrodes was used to record EEG activity (standard 10–20 system; Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz, F3, F4, F7, F8, C3, C4, T3, T4, T5, T6, P3, P4, O1, O2, Fp1, Fp2, Ft7, Ft8, Fc3, Fc4, Tp7, Tp8, Cp3, Cp4, FCz, CPz). Electrodes were filled with electro-gel. All electrodes were referenced to the left mastoid; FPz was used as ground electrode. Horizontal EOG was recorded using electrodes attached to the outer canthi of the eyes, while vertical EOG was recorded from infraorbital and supraorbital electrodes placed in line with the pupil of the left eye. All electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. ERPs were collected using the Neuroscan 4.3 software, and SynAmps amplifiers were used. All signals were continuously sampled at a rate of 1,000 Hz and digitally filtered online with a high-pass filter of 0.10 Hz and a low-pass filter of 100 Hz. Offline continuous signals were epoched into 1,200 ms sweeps including a 200-ms prestimulus baseline. All of the sampled EEG and EOG epochs were baseline corrected and filtered offline using a 30-Hz, 12 dB/oct digital low-pass filter. EEGs were corrected for horizontal and vertical eye movements according to a procedure by Semlitsch et al. (1986). Sweeps containing an artifact exceeding +75 or −75 μV were rejected.

Performance tasks

Divided attention task

The DAT measures the ability to divide attention between two tasks performed simultaneously (Moskowitz 1973). The primary task measures the subjects’ ability to control a displayed error signal in a first-order compensatory tracking task. Error is displayed as a horizontal deviation of a cursor from the midpoint on a horizontal, linear scale. Compensatory joystick movements null the error by returning the cursor to the midpoint. The difficulty of the task is set at a constant level of 50% of the subjects’ maximum capacity, as determined in the training session. Tracking error is measured as the difference in millimeters between the average position of the cursor and the middle of the horizontal scale. In the secondary task, subjects monitor a central display upon which single digits are presented at 1–2 s intervals. The occurrence of the digit “2” is a signal for the subject to remove the foot from a pedal as rapidly as possible. In total, 75 targets and 375 nontargets were presented. Mean absolute tracking error (in millimeters) and number of control losses are the performance measures of the primary task. Number of correct detections and average RT (in milliseconds) are the dependent variables of the secondary task.

In the DAT, two ERP components were calculated, i.e., P100 and P300 (or P3b). P100 reflects early attentional processes and appears around 100 ms above the visual cortex (Mangun 1995; Mangun and Hillyard 1990). The P300, or P3b, has mostly been demonstrated in oddball tasks and appears between 300 and 800 ms in the parietal area (Polich and Kok 1995). It reflects attentional resource allocation and working memory and is affected by the probability of the rare stimulus in both passive and active tasks (Duncan-Johnson 1981; Mertens and Polich 1997; Polich and Kok 1995). The P300 component has also been shown to be an indicator for processing capacity in dual tasks (Brocke et al. 1996; Duncan-Johnson 1981; Isreal et al. 1980; Kok 2001; Smulders et al. 1995; Theunissen et al. 2004; Wickens et al. 1983).

In the present study, ERP averages for individual subjects were calculated for correct responses to targets and distracters of the secondary task of the DAT. Parietal (Pz) P300 peak amplitude and latency were determined in a window between 275 and 650 ms as a measure of the amount of invested capacity in the secondary task. Furthermore, the maximum P100 amplitude and latency at Oz were scored between 80 and 200 ms to examine the effects of primary task load on early attentional processing of stimuli in the secondary task.

Stop signal task

The SST measures motor impulsivity, which is defined as the inability to inhibit a precued response. The current test was adapted from an earlier version of Fillmore et al. (2002). In this task, subjects are required to make quick key responses to visually presented go signals and to inhibit any response when a visual stop signal is suddenly presented. The go signals were four 1.5-cm letters (ABCD) presented one at a time in the center of a computer screen. Subjects were required to respond to each letter as quickly as possible by pressing one of two response buttons. One button is pressed to indicate that “A” or “C” appeared and the other to indicate that “B” or “D” appeared. Letters are displayed for 500 ms and the computer screen was blank for 1.5 s interstimulus interval before the next letter was displayed. The test consisted of 192 trials in which each of the 4 letters were presented equally often. The stop signal occurred in half of the trials and consisted of a visual cue, i.e., “*”, that appeared in one of the four corners of the screen. Subjects were required to withhold any response in case a stop signal was presented. Stop signals were presented 24 times at each of the 4 delays after the onset of a letter: 50, 150, 250, and 350 ms. Trials always began with a 500-ms preparation interval in which a fixation point appeared on the center of the screen. Dependent variables are stop RT and response accuracy. Stop RT is calculated by subtracting the stop signal delay from the RT on go trials associated with n-th percentile of the RT distribution. The n-th percentile corresponds to the percentage of commission errors (not inhibited in stop trial; Logan 1994).

In the SST, the N200 was measured. The negative N200 peak occurs around 200 ms post-stimulus, predominantly in frontal areas, and is reported to be related to response inhibition (Falkenstein et al. 1999; Jodo and Kayama 1992). The N200 amplitude and latency were measured at Fz for correct responses to go trials and for correct inhibitions to the stop signal (four stop signal delays separately), as the minimal amplitude between 150 and 300 ms after the onset of the go signal or each stop signal. As the N200 is an indication of response inhibition, it is expected to be larger in stop conditions.

Subjective high

Directly after smoking and at 45 min post-smoking, a visual analog scale (100 mm) was used by the subject to rate their feeling of “high” as a percentage of the maximum ever experienced (0 indicating not high at all, 100 meaning maximally high).

Pharmacokinetic assessments

Blood samples (6 ml) were taken by venipuncture at baseline, immediately after smoking, and every 15 min during the first hour after smoking. Samples were centrifuged after 30 min and the serum was frozen at −20°C until analyses for pharmacokinetic assessments. THC, THC-COOH, and OH-THC concentrations were determined using solid-phase extraction and gas chromatography with mass spectrometric detection with a limit of quantification of 0.5 ng/ml (for a more detailed description of the method, see Toennes et al. 2010).

Data analyses

Behavioral data were analyzed with a univariate ANOVA with the factors THC (two levels) and cannabis use history (two levels) and their interaction. All ERP parameters were normally distributed and analyzed using ANOVA with the within-subject factors THC (two levels) and stimulus (two levels for DAT and five levels for SST) and the between-subject factor cannabis use history (two levels) and their interactions. All variables were tested two-tailed with a significance level p < 0.05. A Pearson correlation was used to test the relation between behavioral measures, ERP measures, and THC concentrations. All statistical tests were conducted using SPSS (version 15).

Results

Missing data

Due to technical problems, one subject’s foot responses (secondary task) were not registered during the DAT in the THC condition. One subject was not able to inhibit his responses in the SST; therefore, no ERPs could be calculated for this subject (occasional user in the THC condition). ERPs of the SST are also missing for one subject (heavy user in the THC condition), while ERP DAT data from three subjects (one heavy user and two occasional users; all in the THC condition) are missing due to technical malfunctioning.

Subjective high

Mean (SE) values for the subjective high for both groups and treatments are shown in Table 2. In both groups, subjective high was significantly increased immediately and 45 min after smoking THC (F 1,44 = 47.08, p < 0.000 and F 1,43 = 44.81, p < 0.000, respectively). Immediately after smoking, subjective high ratings were also larger in the occasional users compared to heavy users in both THC and placebo condition (F 1,44 = 7.13, p = 0.011).

Table 2 Summary of mean (SE) values of subjective high (immediately and 45 min after smoking) and performance measures in the DAT and SST for heavy and occasional cannabis users

THC concentrations

THC concentrations in serum were overall higher in heavy users compared to occasional users. Occasional users were negative for THC in the placebo condition, whereas heavy users were positive in the placebo condition and at baseline. A summary of the mean (SD) THC, THC-COOH, and OH-THC is given in Table 3.

Table 3 Mean (SD) serum concentrations (in nanograms per milliliter) of THC, THC-COOH, and OH-THC as a function of time after smoking in occasional (THC condition only) and heavy users (THC and placebo condition)

Divided attention task

Mean (SE) values for performance on the DAT are given in Table 2. Behavioral measures of the DAT were significantly influenced by THC. THC increased the number of control losses (F 1,44 = 5.812, p = 0.02) and RT (F 1,43 = 5.724, p = 0.021) and decreased number of hits (F 1,43 = 5.026, p = 0.03). These effects were specific for the occasional users as indicated by a significant or nearly significant THC × cannabis use history interaction for the number of control losses (F 1,44 = 4.900, p = 0.032), RT (F 1,43 = 3.945, p = 0.053), and number of hits (F 1,43 = 4.425, p = 0.042).

P100

ERP analysis included 11 heavy users and 10 occasional users in the THC condition and all subjects in the placebo condition. Mean (SE) values of the ERPs from the DAT are given in Table 4. ERP P100 amplitude demonstrated a significant effect of stimulus (F 1,83 = 6.956, p = 0.01) and THC × cannabis use history interaction (F 1,83 = 7.877, p = 0.006). This interaction indicated that the occasional users showed a decrease in P100 amplitude when under the influence of THC, while in the heavy users, P100 increased when given THC compared to placebo (see Figs. 1 and 2). Separate paired-sample t tests showed that the decrease in P100 in occasional users after THC was significantly different from placebo (t(19) = −2,74, p = 0.013), while the increase in heavy users was not (t(21) = 1,74, p = 0.096). No effects were found for P100 latency in the DAT.

Table 4 Summary of mean (SE) values of the ERPs in the DAT and SST for heavy and occasional cannabis users
Fig. 1
figure 1

Grand average ERP showing P100 at Oz for targets in the DAT. The upper panel shows ERPs of experienced users, while the lower panel shows that of occasional users

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean (SE) P100 (Oz) amplitude during the DAT for heavy and occasional users in the THC and placebo condition

P300

ERP analysis included 11 heavy users and 10 occasional users in the THC condition and all subjects in the placebo condition. P300 amplitude revealed significant main effects of THC (F 1,83 = 9.178, p = 0.003), stimulus (F 1,83 = 53.520, p = 0.000), and THC × stimulus interaction (F 1,83 = 6.364, p = 0.014). No effect of cannabis use history or interaction with cannabis use history was found. P300 amplitudes were largest for targets in the placebo condition (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Grand average ERP showing P300 at Pz for targets in the DAT of both groups of users together

Stop signal task

Mean (SE) values for performance on the SST are given in Table 2. The only behavioral SST measure that was significantly affected by THC is the stop RT. THC significantly increased stop RT (F 1,44 = 5.123, p = 0.029). No effect of history of drug use or interaction was found on the behavioral measures.

N200

No THC or cannabis use history effects were found on the SST frontal N200. A main effect of stimulus type was found on N200 amplitude (F 4,15 = 56,491, p = 0.000) and latency (F 4,15 = 683,429, p = 0.000), showing the largest and slowest N200 peaks for the stop trials with the longest delays. Mean (SE) values of the ERPs from the SST are given in Table 4.

Correlations

The stop RT in the SST was positively correlated with THC concentration measured before performance of the SST (r(46) = 0.37, p = 0.012) and after completing the SST (r(45) = 0.38, p = 0.011). Other correlations between ERP measures, behavioral results, subjective high, or THC concentrations were not significant.

Discussion

Previous studies have demonstrated that heavy cannabis users develop tolerance for the impairing effects of cannabis on cognitive performance. In the present study, we used ERPs to differentiate the information processes underlying the different performances of occasional and heavy cannabis users in cognitive tasks. The results of this study demonstrate that a drug use history by THC interaction was only present in the early P100 component which was measured during a DAT. In occasional users, the P100 amplitude was found to be decreased in the THC condition compared to the placebo condition. The P100 is thought to represent early attentional selection (Taylor 2002) and has been found to be enhanced when distracters are suppressed (Coull 1998). It could, therefore, be argued that, in the present study, the occasional cannabis users are less able to differentiate the targets from the distracters when under the influence of THC, and hence, their P100 is decreased. Since the heavy users do not demonstrate a change in P100 amplitude after THC administration, this could be an indication of tolerance to the effects of THC.

Cannabis also had an effect on the P300 of the DAT, i.e., amplitude was decreased after THC treatment. However, no interaction was found with drug use history. It thus seems that experienced users of THC were not tolerant to the effect of THC on P300. Previous studies using ERPs have also demonstrated that P300 is a robust measure of THC intoxication. Ilan et al. (2004) studied ERPs while subjects were in an intoxicated state. They showed that acute administration of a low dose of cannabis (3.45% THC) impaired performance on a working memory task and an episodic memory task, while at the same time amplitudes of the related ERP components were decreased (N100, P200, P300, and N400). Although this study only included casual cannabis smokers (defined as using between once a month and once a week during the last year), the authors did notice that the degree of intoxication (measured by subjective high) influenced the ERP, in that the most intoxicated subject also showed the most decreased amplitudes. Recently, Böcker et al. (2010) concluded that ERP components related to selective attention were not influenced by high doses of cannabis (29.3, 49.2, and 69.4 mg), whereas amplitudes of components reflecting perceptual processing as well as stimulus classification (P300) were decreased. In this study, ERPs were measured 2 h after the administration of THC in regular nondaily users. These previous studies, together with the current results, demonstrate that P300 is a particular sensitive measure of THC-induced intoxication.

In the present study, no effects of THC for either of the groups were found on the ERP component of the SST, whereas the behavioral data did demonstrate increased stop RT in both heavy and occasional users. It is possible that the SST used is not well suited to elicit clear inhibition of ERPs. For instance, it included 96 go and 96 stop signals, which may be too little to elicit a clear and reliable N200. In our previous study, we demonstrated that heavy users only demonstrated impairment of impulse control in the SST when THC concentrations were above 10 ng/ml in serum. In the present study, we also demonstrated that THC concentration was positively related with stop signal RT, which measures impulse control.

The present data indicate that heavy cannabis users did not show tolerance to THC effects on subjective high. In addition, occasional cannabis users felt more high after THC than heavy users, although THC serum concentrations in heavy cannabis users were much higher than those observed in occasional users. A single dose of 500 μg/kg THC produced a mean peak concentration of 120.9 ng/ml in heavy cannabis users and 49.1 ng/ml in occasional users. Both groups were instructed to adhere to the same smoking protocol. On average, occasional and heavy users smoked 28 and 22 number of puffs of the THC cigarette. Therefore, present differences in THC concentrations seem to indicate that puff volume of occasional and heavy cannabis users may have differed and that heavy cannabis users needed higher THC concentrations to achieve their desired high.

The current study appears to be the first to compare acute effects of a high THC dose on ERPs in heavy and occasional cannabis users. Our data confirm previous studies in showing that P300 is decreased after cannabis consumption, in occasional as well as heavy cannabis users. In addition, we found that, at early attentional selection processes, occasional cannabis users show a decreased P100, whereas heavy users have developed tolerance. These findings are in line with the behavioral data that were demonstrated. THC significantly impaired performance of occasional users on the DAT and the SST, whereas heavy users were only impaired on the SST. Therefore, it can be concluded that heavy cannabis users have developed tolerance for some of the effects of THC and that this tolerance is also demonstrated at the level of brain processing, as was shown with the P100.