Skip to main content
Log in

Rule-Governed Behavior: Teaching a Preliminary Repertoire of Rule-Following to Children With Autism

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rule-governed behavior is generally considered an integral component of complex verbal repertoires but has rarely been the subject of empirical research. In particular, little or no previous research has attempted to establish rule-governed behavior in individuals who do not already display the repertoire. This study consists of two experiments that evaluated multiple exemplar training procedures for teaching a simple component skill, which may be necessary for developing a repertoire of rule-governed behavior. In both experiments, children with autism were taught to respond to simple rules that specified antecedents and the behaviors that should occur in their presence. In the first study, participants were taught to respond to rules containing “if/then” statements, where the antecedent was specified before the behavior. The second experiment was a replication and extension of the first. It involved a variation on the manner in which rules were presented. Both experiments eventually demonstrated generalization to novel rules for all participants; however variations to the standard procedure were required for several participants. Results suggest that rule-following can be analyzed and taught as generalized operant behavior and implications for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., O’Hora, D., Roche, B., Hayes, S. C., Bisset, R. T., & Liddy, F. (2001). Understanding and verbal regulation. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition (pp. 103–118). New York, NY: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. (2001). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry: II. The Psychological Record, 51, 589–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., B Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis. (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a multiple-stimulus format for assessing reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 519–533.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of naming in children with no prior tact responses as a function of multiple exemplar histories of instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdez, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J. (1991). Substitution and reference. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior: The First International Institute on Verbal Relations (pp. 3–14). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (Ed.). (1989). Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Blackledge, J. T., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Language and cognition: Constructing an alternative approach within the behavioral tradition. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 3–20). New York, NY: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Fox, E., Gifford, E. V., Wilson, K. G., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001). Derived relational responding as learned behavior. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 21–50). New York, NY: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Home, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeBlanc, L. A., Miguel, C. F., Cummings, A. R., Goldsmith, T. R, & Carr, J. E. (2003). The effects of three stimulus-equivalence testing conditions on emergent US geography relations of children diagnosed with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 279–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, B. A., Catania, A. C., & Shimoff, E. (1985). Effects of uninstructed verbal behavior on nonverbal responding: Contingency descriptions versus performance descriptions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 155–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. (2004). Derived relational networks and control by novel instructions: A possible model of generative verbal responding. The Psychological Record, 54, 437–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, L. J. (1984). Listening and understanding. Behavior Analyst, 7, 29–39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Mathews, B. A. (1981). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 207–220.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Mathews, B. A., & Catania, A. C. (1986). Human operant performance: Sensitivity and pseudosensitivity to contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 149–157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group and the B. F. Skinner Foundation.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York, NY: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tarbox, J., Tarbox, R. S., & O’Hora, D. (2009). Nonrelational and relational instructional control. In R. A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes (Eds.), Derived relational responding: Applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities (pp. 111–127). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan Tarbox PhD, BCBA-D.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tarbox, J., Zuckerman, C.K., Bishop, M.R. et al. Rule-Governed Behavior: Teaching a Preliminary Repertoire of Rule-Following to Children With Autism. Analysis Verbal Behav 27, 125–139 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393096

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393096

Key words

Navigation