Skip to main content
Log in

Matching Theory in Natural Human Environments

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Matching theory is a mathematical account of behavior, many aspects of which have been confirmed in laboratory experiments with nonhuman and human subjects. The theory asserts that behavior is distributed across concurrently available response alternatives in the same proportion that reinforcement is distributed across those alternatives. The theory also asserts that behavior on a single response alternative is a function not only of reinforcement contingent on that behavior, but also of reinforcement contingent on other behaviors and of reinforcement delivered independently of behavior. These assertions constitute important advances in our understanding of the effects of reinforcement on behavior. Evidence from the applied literature suggests that matching theory holds not only in laboratory environments, but also in natural human environments. In addition, the theory has important therapeutic implications. For example, it suggests four new intervention strategies, and it can be used to improve treatment planning and management. Research on matching theory illustrates the progression from laboratory experimentation with nonhuman subjects to therapeutic applications in natural human environments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ayllon, T., Layman, D., & Kandel, H. (1975). A behavioral-educational alternative to drug control of hyperactive children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 137–146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ayllon, T., & Roberts, M. (1974). Eliminating discipline problems by strengthening academic performance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 71–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 231–242.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1979). Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in studies of choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 32, 269–281.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1983). Matching, statistics, and common sense. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 499–501.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M., & Rachlin, H. C. (1969). Choice as time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 861–874.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., & Bevan, P. (1976). Behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 135–141.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., & Bevan, P. (1977). Effect of punishment on human variable-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 275–279.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., & Bevan, P. (1978). Effect of variable-interval punishment on the behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 161–166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brawley, E., Harris, F., Allen, K., Fleming, R., & Peterson, R. (1969). Behavior modification of an autistic child. Behavioral Science, 14, 87–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brigham, T., Finfrock, S., Breunig, M., & Bushell, D. (1972). The use of programmed materials in the analysis of academic contingencies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 177–182.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, E. G., & McDowell, J. J. (1980). Social control of self-injurious behavior of organic etiology. Behavior Therapy, 11, 402–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1981). Discussion: The flight from experimental analysis. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, & C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantificaiton of steady-state operant behaviour (pp. 49–64). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cliffe, M. J., & Parry, S. J. (1980). Matching to reinforcer value: Human concurrent variable-interval performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 557–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conger, R., & Killeen, P. (1974). Use of concurrent operants in small group research: A demonstration. Pacific Sociological Review, 17, 399–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cullen, C. (1981). The flight to the laboratory. The Behavior Analyst, 4, 81–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, P. A. (1977). Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation of the law of effect. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 233–287). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, P. A., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1976). Toward a law of response strength. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1131–1153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epling, W. F., & Pierce, W. D. (1983). Applied behavior analysis: New directions from the laboratory. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 27–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1978). Is operant conditioning getting bored with behavior? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 347–349.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glynn, E. (1970). Classroom applications of self-determined reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 3, 123–132.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 267–272.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 243–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. R. (1982). Incentive theory. In D. J. Bernstein (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1981: Vol. 29. Response structure and organization. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, F., & Shields, F. (1972). Modification of arithmetic response rate and attending behavior in a seventh-grade student. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 79–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O., Freitag, G., Gold, V., & Kassorla, I. (1965). Experimental studies in childhood schizophrenia: Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2, 67–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O., Litrownik, A., & Mann, R. (1971). Response latencies to auditory stimuli in autistic children engaged in self-stimulatory behavior. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 9, 39–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marholin, D., II, & Steinman, W. (1977). Stimulus control in the classroom as a function of the behavior reinforced. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 465–478.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J (1981a). On the validity and utility of Herrnstein’s hyperbola in applied behavior analysis. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, & C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantification of steady-state operant behaviour (pp. 311–324). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J (1981b). Wilkinson’s method of estimating the parameters of Herrnstein’s hyperbola. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 413–414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J (1982). The importance of Herrnstein’s mathematical statement of the law of effect for behavior therapy. American Psychologist, 37, 771–779.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J (1986). On the falsifiability of matching theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 63–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J, & Wixted, J. T. (1988). The linear system theory’s account of behavior maintained by variable-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 143–169.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J, & Wood, H. M. (1984). Confirmation of linear system theory prediction: Changes in Herrnstein’s k as a function of changes in reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 183–192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. J, & Wood, H. M. (1985). Confirmation of linear system theory prediction: Rate of change of Herrnstein’s A: as a function of response-force requirement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 61–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mullins, E., Agunwamba, C. C., & Donohoe, A. J. (1982). On the analysis of studies of choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 323–327.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Myerson, J., & Hale, S. (1984). Practical implications of the matching law. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 367–380.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G., & Reid, J. (1970). Reciprocity and coercion: Two facets of social systems. In C. Neuringer & J. Michael (Eds.), Behavior modification in clinical psychology (pp. 133–177). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, W. D., & Epling, W. F. (1980). What happened to analysis in applied behavior analysis? The Behavior Analyst, 3, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Poling, A., Picker, M., Grossett, D., Hall-Johnson, E., & Holbrook, M. (1981). The schism between experimental and applied behavior analysis: Is it real and who cares? The Behavior Analyst, 4, 93–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. C., & Baum, W. M. (1972). Effects of alternative reinforcement: Does the source matter? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 231–241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sajwaj, T., Twardosz, S., & Burke, M. (1972). Side effects of extinction procedures in a remedial preschool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 163–175.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. L. (1983). Skinner’s environmentalism: The analogy with natural selection. Behaviorism, 11, 133–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H., & Burgess, I. S. (1982). Matching since Baum (1979). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 339–348.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Winett, R., & Roach, E. (1973). The effects of reinforcing academic performance on social behavior. Psychological Record, 23, 391–396.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This article is based on an address delivered at the Fourth Annual Meeting of the Southeastern Association for Behavior Analysis in Asheville, NC, October, 1987.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McDowell, J.J. Matching Theory in Natural Human Environments. BEHAV ANALYST 11, 95–109 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392462

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392462

Navigation