Skip to main content
Log in

Averbia as a selective naming disorder: A single case report

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

After a moderate head injury a 33-year-old woman presented a selective defect in finding verbs and name actions. Objects, colors, body parts, and qualities were named in a normal way. No other associated aphasic defects were observed. It was proposed that the selection and use of verbs in their correct forms implies two different aspects, which eventually can become dissociated: (1) the ability to retrieve the lexical item (the action name); and (2) the ability to use the correct affixation, that is, to select the morphological form according to the current context. Only the second would be associated with agrammatism. Defects in the first aspect (selection of the lexical item) would represent a particular form of anomia, a category-specific anomia, or anomia for verbs (averbia). Our patient illustrated this particular type of category-specific naming disorder.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (1992).Neuropsicologia clinica [Clinical neuropsychology]. Medellin, Colombia: Prensa Creativa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A. (1994).Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish-speaker. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, D. F. (1988). Classic syndromes of aphasia. In F. Boller, J. Grafman, G. Rizzolatti, & H. Goodglass (Eds.),Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 1, pp. 267–280). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, R. S. (1988). Category-specific deficits in aphasia.Aphasiology, 2, 237–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. (1991). Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain.Nature, 349, 788–790.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Renzi, E. & Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token Test.Cortex, 14, 327–342.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983).Evaluación de las afasias y de transtornos similares (2nd ed.) Buenos Aires: Editorial Médica Panamericana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., Klein, B., Carey, O., & Jones, K. (1966). Specific semantic word categories in aphasia.Cortex, 2, 74–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J., Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A. (1985). Category-specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction.Nature, 316, 439–440.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolicoeur, R., Gluck, M. A., & Kossly, S. (1984). Pictures and names, making the connection.Cognitive Psychology, 16, 243–275.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E., Goddglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983).The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A. (1985). Aphasia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.),Handbook of clinical neurology: Clinical neuropsychology (Vol. 45, pp. 287–332). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kremin, H. (1988). Naming and its disorders. In F. Boller, J. Grafman, G. Rizzolatti, & H. Goodglass (Eds.),Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 1, pp. 307–328). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1966).Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, P. & Campbell, R. (1992). Optic aphasia with spared action naming: A description and possible loci of impairment.Neuropsychologia, 30, 587–592.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, R., & Warrington, E. K. (1985). Category specificity in an agrammatic patient: The relative impairment of verb retrieval and comprehension.Neuropsychologia, 23, 709–727.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, P., & Warrington, E. K. (1978). Category-specific preservation: A single case study.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 41, 571–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Florez, A., & Castro, C. (1990). Normative data on the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination in a Spanish-speaking population.Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 313–322.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, C. M. (1986). Anomia for animals in a child.Brain, 109, 1225–1242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K. (1981). Neuropsychological studies of verbal semantic systems.Philosphical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B295, 411–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K. & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia.Brain, 106, 859–878.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments.Brain, 107, 829–854.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for clinical use.Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zingeser, L. B., & Berndt, R. S. (1988). Grammatical class and context effects in a case of pure anomia: Implications for models of lexical processing.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 473–516.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ardila, A., Rosselli, M. Averbia as a selective naming disorder: A single case report. J Psycholinguist Res 23, 139–148 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02143920

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02143920

Keywords

Navigation