Skip to main content
Log in

Memory of distances and directions moved as cues during temporary spiral construction in the spiderLeucauge mariana (Araneae: Araneidae)

  • Published:
Journal of Insect Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The spider Leucauge marianaprobably uses the presence of temporary spiral (TSP) lines already in place to determine sites of attachment of currently produced loops of temporary spiral, but less rigidly than has been previously supposed for orb weavers. Memory of distances and directions traveled recently to and from TSP lines is implicated by the fact that adjustments to experimental and natural discontinuities in previous TSP lines occur gradually rather than being abrupt. Distances and directions traveled along both radii and previous TSP lines correlate with relative amounts of adjustment. Body size may also be used as a reference measure, but not in the simple, inflexible way suggested by R. W. G. Hingston (A Naturalist in Himalaya,Small, Maynard, Boston, 1920). Tensions on radii are not used as cues in any simple way, and may not be used at all, since experimental changes in tensions produced effects consistent with resulting changes in thread positions but inconsistent with tension differences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barth, F. G., and Seyfarth, E. A. (1971). Slit sense organs and kinesthetic orientation.Z.Vergl. Physiol. 74: 326–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, W. J. (1985). Sources of information controlling motor patterns in arthropod local search orientation.J. Insect Physiol. 31: 837–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, P. H. (1984). Prey detection by the sand scorpion.Sci. Am. 251(6): 94–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchli, H. H. R. (1969). Hunting behavior in the Ctenizidae.Am. Zool. 9: 175–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, J. D. (1984). Orientation in a vertical plane: The use of light cues by an orb-weaving spider,Araneus diadematus Clerk.Anim. Behav. 32: 162–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R., and Dawkins, M. (1973). Decisions and the uncertainty of behaviour.Behaviour 45: 83–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny, M. (1976). The physical properties of spider's silk and their role in the design of orb webs.J. Exp. Biol. 65: 483–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1972). The web ofUloborus diversus (Araneae: Uloboridae).J. Zool. Lond. 166: 417–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1974). The “inverted ladder” web ofScoloderus sp. and the intermediate orb ofEustala (?) sp. (Araneidae).J. Nat. Hist. 9: 93–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1976). Photography of orb webs in the field.Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 3: 200–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1981). Construction behaviour and the distribution of tensions in orb webs.Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 5: 189–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1982). Behavioral characteristics for the highest classification of orb-weaving spiders.Evol. 36: 1067–1095.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1986). Effects of orb web geometry on prey interception and retention. In Shear, W. A. (ed.),Spiders Webs Behavior and Evolution, Stanford University Press, Palo Alto, Calif., pp. 70–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1987a). Web-building behavior of anapid, symphytognathid, and mysmenid spiders (Araneae).J. Arachnol. (in press).

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1987b). Effects of gravity on temporary spiral construction byLeucauge mariana (Araneae: Araneidae).J. Ethol. (in press).

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1987c). Flexibility in innate behavior: Effects of gland contents on orb web design (in preparation).

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1987d). Imprecision in innate behavior (in preparation).

  • Eberhard, W. G. (1987e). Construction of the first loop of sticky spiral byLeucauge mariana (Araneae: Araneidae) (in preparation).

  • Görner, P. (1973). Beispiele einer Orientierung ohne richtende Aussenreize.Fortschr. Zool. 21: 20–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Görner, P., and Zeppenfeld, C. (1980). The runs of Pardosa amentata (Araneae, Lycosidae) after removing its cocoon.Proc. 8 Int. Cong. Arachnol., pp. 243–248.

  • Griffin, D. R. (1984).Animal Thinking, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hingston, R. W. G. (1920).A Naturalist in Himalaya, Small, Maynard, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • König, M. (1951). Beitrage zur Kenntnis des Netzbaues orbiteiler Spinnen.Z. Tierpsychol. 8: 462–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Land, M. F. (1985). The morphology and optics of spider eyes. In Barth, F. G. (ed.),Neurobiology of Arachnids, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 53–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeGuelte, L. (1969). Learning in spiders.Am. Zool. 9: 145–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCook, H. (1889).American Spiders and Their Spinning-work. I. Webs and Nests, Author, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, P. (1970). Orb web construction: Interaction of spider (Araneus diadematus Cl.) and thread configuration.Anim. Behav. 18: 478–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savory, T. H. (1952).The Spider's Web, Frederick Warne, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt, P. N. (1965). Do we live in the best of all worlds? Spider webs suggest an answer.Perspect. Biol. Med. 8: 475–487.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt, P. N., Reed, C. F., and Peakall, D. B. (1968).A Spider's Web, Springer Verlag, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Eberhard, W.G. Memory of distances and directions moved as cues during temporary spiral construction in the spiderLeucauge mariana (Araneae: Araneidae). J Insect Behav 1, 51–66 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01052503

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01052503

Key words

Navigation