Skip to main content
Log in

Tongue-flicking and biting in response to chemical food stimuli by an iguanid lizard (Dipsosaurus dorsalis) having sealed vomeronasal ducts: Vomerolfaction may mediate these behavioral responses

  • Published:
Journal of Chemical Ecology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the iguanid lizardDipsosaurus dorsalis, chemical food stimuli were discriminated from other odorants by vomerolfaction. This was demonstrated in a 2 × 3 experiment in which groups of lizards with sealed vomeronasal ducts or sham-sealed vomeronasal ducts responded to carrot chemical stimuli, cologne, and distilled water presented on cotton-tipped applicators. Abilities to detect and discriminate food chemicals were abolished in lizards having sealed vomeronasal ducts. For tongue-flick attack score and number of lizards biting, the sham-sealed group responded more strongly to carrot stimuli than to the control stimuli, but the group having sealed ducts did not. Lizards having sham-sealed ducts responded more strongly to carrot stimuli than did lizards having sealed ducts; responses by the two groups of lizards to control stimuli did not differ. Tongue-flicking occurred when the vomeronasal system detected a chemical stimulus from either carrot or cologne. Biting occurred only when the vomeronasal organ detected food stimuli (from carrot). Most duct-sealed lizards opened their mouths, some repeatedly. Mouth-opening thus occurs when the vomeronasal organ does not detect chemicals. It may be an attempt to stimulate or prime the vomeronasal organ or to dislodge the sealant.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alberts, A.C. 1989. Ultraviolet visual sensitivity in desert iguanas: Implications for pheromone detection.Anim. Behav. 37:129–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G.M. 1967. Chemical-cue preferences of inexperienced snakes: Comparative aspects.Science 157:718–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G.M. 1970. Chemical perception in reptiles, pp. 241–308,in J.W. Johnston, D.G. Moulton, and A. Turk (eds.). Advances in Chemoreception, Vol. 1. Communication by Chemical Signals. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G.M. 1980. Behavioral and stimulus correlates of vomeronasal functioning in reptiles: Feeding, grouping, sex, and tongue use, pp. 275–301,in D. Muller-Schwarze and R.M. Silverstein (eds.). Chemical Signals: Vertebrates and Aquatic Invertebrates. Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G.M., andHess, E.H. 1968. Factors influencing the chemical release of prey attack in newborn snakes.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 66:289–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G.M., andPruitt, C.H. 1975. Role of the tongue and senses in feeding of naive and experienced garter snakes.Physiol. Behav. 14:185–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, J., Maxion, R., Sharps, M., Weiss, D., O'Connell, B., andChiszar, D. 1982. Predatory behavior in a congenitally alingual Russell's viper (Vipera russelli). 1. Strike-induced chemosensory searching.Bull. Md. Herpol. Soc. 18:196–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiszar, D., andScudder, K.M. 1980. Chemosensory searching by rattlesnakes during predatory episodes, pp. 125–139,in D. Muller-Schwarze and R.M. Silverstein (eds.). Chemical Signals: Vertebrates and Aquatic Invertebrates. Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr. 1989a. Absence of prey odor discrimination by iguanid and agamid lizards in applicator tests.Copeia 1989:472–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr. 1989b. Prey odor discrimination in varanoid lizards: Responses of gila monsters (Heloderma suspectum) and savannah monitors (Varanus exanthematicus) to chemical stimuli presented on cotton swabs.Ethology 81:250–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr. 1990a. Prey odor detection by teiid and lacertid lizards and its relationship to foraging mode in lizard families.Copeia 1990:237–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr. 1990b. Prey odor discrimination by anguid lizards.Herpetologica. 46:183–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andAlberts, A.C. 1990. Responses to chemical food stimuli by an herbivorous actively foraging lizard,Dipsosaurus dorsalis.Herpetologica. 46:259–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andBurghardt, G.M. 1990a. A comparative analysis of scoring methods for chemical discrimination of prey chemicals by squamate reptiles.J. Chem. Ecol. 16:45–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andBurghardt, G.M. 1990b. Vomerolfaction and vomodor.J. Chem. Ecol. 16:103–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andVitt, L.J. 1984a. Conspecific odor detection by the male broad-headed skink,Eumeces laticeps: Effects of sex and site of odor source and of male reproductive condition.J. Exp. Zool. 230:199–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., andVitt, L.J. 1984b. Detection of conspecific odors by the female broad-headed skink,Eumeces laticeps.J. Exp. Zool. 229:49–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andVitt, L.J. 1986a. Interspecific odour discrimination by a lizard (Eumeces laticeps).Anim. Behav. 34:367–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andVitt, L.J. 1986b. Tracking of female conspecific odor trails by male broad-headed skinks (Eumeces laticeps).Ethology 71:242–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., andVitt, L.J. 1989. Prey odor discrimination by the broad-headed skink (Eumeces laticeps).J. Exp. Zool. 249:11–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W.E., Jr., Garstka, W.R., andVitt, L.J. 1986. Female sex pheromone in the lizardEumeces laticeps.Herpetologica 42:361–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowles, R.B., andPhelan, R.L. 1958. Olfaction in rattlesnakes.Copeia 1958: 77–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dial, B.E., Weldon, P.J., andCurtis, B. 1989. Predator-prey signals: Chemosensory identification of snake predators by gekkonid lizards and its ecological consequences.J. Herpetol. 23:224–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duvall, D. 1979. Western fence lizard (Sceloporus occidentalis) chemical signals. 1. Conspecific discriminations and release of a species-typical visual display.J. Exp. Zool. 210:321–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duvall, D. 1981. Western fence lizard (Sceloporus occidentalis) chemical signals. II. A replication with naturally breeding adults and a test of the Cowles and Pheian hypothesis of rattlesnake olfaction.J. Exp. Zool. 218:351–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duvall, D., Herskowitz, R., andTrupiano-Duvall, J. 1980. Responses of five-lined skinks (Eumeces fasciatus) and ground skinks (Scincella lateralis) to conspecific and interspecific chemical cues.J. Herpetol. 14:21–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, N.B. 1982. Species-specificity of sex pheromone trails of sympatric and allopatric garter snakes (Thamnophis).Copeia 1982:10–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garstka, W.R., andCrews, D. 1981. Female sex pheromone in the skin and circulation of a garter snake.Science 214:681–683.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, B.M., andDuvall, D. 1983. Occurrence and function of prairie rattlesnake mouth gaping in a non-feeding context.J. Exp. Zool. 227:471–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, B.M., andDuvall, D. 1985. Mouth gaping and head shaking by prairie rattlesnakes are associated with vomeronasal organ olfaction.Copeia 1985:496–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, B.M., andHalpern, M. 1989. Chemical access to the vomeronasal organs of the lizardChalcides ocellatus.J. Exp. Zool. 249:150–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, B.M., andHalpern, M. 1990. Vomeronasal organ chemoreception in tongue-flicking, exploratory, and feeding behaviour of the lizard,Chalcides ocellatus. Anim. Behav. 39:692–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, M. 1987. The organization and function of the vomeronasal system.Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 10:325–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, M., andFrumin, N. 1979. Roles of the vomeronasal and olfactory systems in prey attack and feeding in adult garter snakes.Physiol. Behav. 22:1183–1189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, M., andKubie, J.L. 1980. Chemical access to the vomeronasal organs of garter snakes.Physiol Behav. 24:367–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, M., andKubie, J.L. 1983. Snake tongue flicking behavior: clues to vomeronasal system functioning, pp. 45–72,in D. Muller-Schwarze and R.M. Silverstein (eds.). Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 3. Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, S.B., andHalpern, M. 1982. Laboratory observations of aggregative behavior of garter snakes,Thamnophis sirtalis: Roles of visual, olfactory, and vomeronasal senses.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 96:984–999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollander, A., andWolfe, D.A. 1973. Nonparametric Statistical Methods. John Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krekorian, C.O. 1989. Field and laboratory observations on chemoreception in the desert iguanaDipsosaurus dorsalis. J. Herpetol. 23:267–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubie, J.L., Vagvolgyi, A., andHalpern, M. 1978. Roles of vomeronasal and olfactory systems in courtship behavior of male garter snakes.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 92:627–641.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, S.B. 1972. The evolution of the tongue of snakes, and its bearing on snake origins, pp. 191–273,in T. Dobzhansky, M.K. Hecht and W.C. Steere (eds.). Evolutionary Biology, Vol. 6. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgans, L.F., andHeidt, G.A. 1978. Comparative tongue histology and scanning electron microscopy of the diamondback water snake (Natrix rhombifera) and black rat snake (Elaphe obsoleta) (Reptilia, Serpentes, Colubridae).J. Herpetol. 12:275–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pederson, J.M. 1988. Laboratory observations on the function of tongue extrusion in the desert iguana (Dipsosaurus dorsalis).J. Comp. Psychol. 102:193–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwenk, K. 1985. Occurrence, distribution and functional significance of taste buds in lizards.Copeia 1985:91–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S. 1956. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, C.A. 1983. A review of lizard chemoreception, pp. 119–133,in R.B. Huey, E.R. Pianka, and T.W. Schoener (eds.). Lizard Ecology: Studies of a Model Organism. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, C.A., andMoakley, G.P. 1985. Chemoreception inSceloporus jarrovi: Does olfaction activate the vomeronasal system?Copeia 1985:239–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoen, C., Bauwens, D., andVerheyen, R. 1986. Chemoreceptive and behavioral responses of the common lizardLacerta vivipara to snake chemical deposits.Anim. Behav. 34:1805–1813.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, P.J. 1982. Responses to ophiophagous snakes by snakes of the genusThamnophis.Copeia 1982:788–794.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, D., Baker, E.M., Gonzales, E. Del C., andSosa, I.R. 1987. Kinship recognition and grouping in hatchling green iguanas.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21:83–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilde, W.S. 1938. The role of Jacobson's organs in the feeding reaction of the common garter snake,Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis (Linn.).J. Exp. Zool. 77:445–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B.J. 1962. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cooper, W.E., Alberts, A.C. Tongue-flicking and biting in response to chemical food stimuli by an iguanid lizard (Dipsosaurus dorsalis) having sealed vomeronasal ducts: Vomerolfaction may mediate these behavioral responses. Journal of Chemical Ecology 17, 135–146 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00994427

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00994427

Key Words

Navigation