Skip to main content
Log in

Comparison of the effects of four cholinomimetic agents on cognition in primates following disruption by scopolamine or by lists of objects

  • Original Investigations
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ability of four central cholinomimetics to reverse a scopolamine-induced spatial memory impairment or to improve visual recognition memory in primates was examined. Physostigmine (0.04–0.08 mg/kg IM) fully reversed the effects of scopolamine (0.03 mg/kg). Coadministration of pilocarpine (3.0–5.0 mg/kg) caused partial reversal of the scopolamine impairment after intermediate or long retention intervals (10 or 20 s). Treatment with arecoline (0.1–1.8 mg/kg) or nicotine (1.0–2.0 mg/kg) generally did not reverse the effects of scopolamine. A task in which memory could be taxed by increasing the number of visual stimuli presented appeared more sensitive to the effects of cholinomimetics on cognition than the scopolamine reversal model. In this paradigm treatment with physostigmine (0.001, 0.01 or 0.03 mg/kg) increased choice accuracy from about 55 to 70% correct. Arecoline improved performance at one dose only (0.1 mg/kg) which also induced marked adverse side-effects (salivation and tremor). Pilocarpine improved performance in the dose range 0.125–0.35 mg/kg, but not at higher doses which also induced marked salivation. Treatment with nicotine (0.001–2.0 mg/kg tended to improve performance but this did not reach statistical significance. The relevance of these findings for studies in man and for animal models of dementia is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aigner TG, Mishkin M (1986) The effects of physostigmine and scopolamine on recognition memory in monkeys. Behav Neural Biol 45:81–87

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson K, Post B (1974) Effects of cigarette smoking on verbal rote learning and physiological arousal. Scand J Psychol 15:263–267

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartus RT (1978) Evidence for a direct cholinergic involvement in the scopolamine-induced amnesia in monkeys: effects of concurrent administration of physostigmine and methylphenidate with scopolamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 9:833–836

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartus RT (1979) Physostigmine and recent memory: effects in young and aged nonhuman primates. Science 206:1087–1089

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartus RT, Johnson HR (1976) Short-term memory in the rhesus monkey: disruption from the anticholinergic scopolamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 5:39–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartus RT, Dean RL, Beer B (1980) Memory deficits in aged Cebus monkeys and facilitation with central cholinomimetics. Neurobiol Aging 1:145–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Caine ED (1980) Cholinomimetic treatment fails to impove memory disorder. New Engl J Med 303:585–586

    Google Scholar 

  • Christie JE, Shering A, Ferguson J, Glen AIM (1981) Physostigmine and arecoline: effects of intravenous infusions in Alzheimer presenile dementia. Br J Psychiatry 138:46–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Corkin S (1982) Some relationships between global amnesia and the memory impairments in Alzheimer's disease. In: Corkin S, Davies K, Growdon JH, Usdin E, Wurtman RJ (eds) Alzheimers disease: A report of progress in research, vol 19. Raven Press, New York, pp 149–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies P, Verth AH (1978) Regional distribution of muscarinic acetylcholine receptors in normal and Alzheimer's type dementia brains. Brain Res 138:385–392

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis KL, Mohs RC, Tinklenberg JR, Pfefferbaum A, Hollister LE, Kopell BS (1978) Physostigmine: improvement of longterm memory processes in normal humans. Science 201:272–274

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis KL, Mohs RC, Davis BM, Levy M, Rosenberg GS, Horvath TB, De Nigris Y, Ross A, Decker P, Rothpearl A (1981) Cholinomimetic agents and human memory: clinical studies in Alzheimer's disease and scopolamine dementia. In: Crook T, Gershon S (eds) Strategies for the development of an effective treatment for senile dementia. Mark Powley, Connecticut, pp 53–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Elrod K, Buccafusco JJ, Jackson WJ (1988) Nicotine enhances delayed matching-to-sample performance by primates. Life Sci 43:277–287

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans HL (1975) Scopolamine effects on visual discrimination: modifications related to stimulus control. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 195:105–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Flicker C, Bartus RT, Crook TH, Ferris SH (1984) Effects of aging and dementia upon recent visuospatial memory. Neurobiol Aging 5:275–283

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman M, Oscar-Berman M (1986) Selective delayed response deficits in Parkinson's and Alzheimer's disease. Arch Neurol 43:886–890

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman SB, Harley EA, Iversen LL (1988) Biochemical measurement of muscarinic receptor efficacy and its role in receptor regulation. TIPS 9:54–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman SB, Harley EA, Patel S (1989) Direct measurement of muscarinic agents in the central nervous system of mice using “ex vivo” binding. Manuscript submitted Eur J Pharmacol

  • Heise GA, Milar KS (1984) Drugs and stimulus control. In: Iversen L, Iversen SD, Snyder SH (eds) Handbook of psychopharmacology, vol 18. Plenum Press, New York, pp 129–190

    Google Scholar 

  • Irle E, Kessler J, Markowitsch HJ, Hofman W (1988) Primate learning tasks reveal strong impairment in patients with presenile or senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain Cogn 6:429–449

    Google Scholar 

  • Ksir C (1975) Scopolamine and amphetamine effects on discrimination: interaction wth stimulus control. Psychopharmacology 43:37–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris RG, Kopelman MD (1986) The memory deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia: a review. Q J Exp Psychol 38A:575–602

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss MB, Albert MS, Butters N, Payne M (1986) Differential patterns of memory loss among patients with Alzheimer's disease, Huntington's disease and alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome. Arch Neurol 43:239–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Newhouse PA, Sunderland T, Tariot PN, Blumhardt CL, Weingartner H, Mellow A, Murphy DL (1988) Intravenous nicotine in Alzheimer's disease: a pilot study. Psychopharmacology 95:171–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry EK, Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, Bergmann K, Gibson PH, Perry RH (1978) Correlation of cholinergic abnormalities with senile plaques and mental test scores in senile dementia. Br Med J ii:1457–1459

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley RM, Bowes PM, Baker HF, Cross TJ (1984) An involvement of acetylcholine in object discrimination learning and memory in the marmoset. Neuropsychologia 22:253–263

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley RM, Murray TK, Johnson JA, Baker HF (1986) Learning impairment following lesion of the basal nucleus of Meynert in the marmoset: modification by cholinergic drugs. Brain Res 376:108–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley RM, Baker HF, Drewett B (1987) Effects of arecoline and pilocarpine on learning ability in marmosets pretreated with hemicholinium-3. Psychopharmacology 91:512–514

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley RM, Samson NA, Baker HF, Johnson JA (1988) Visuospatial learning impairment following lesion of the cholinergic projection to the hippocampus. Brain Res 456:71–87

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossor M, Iversen LL (1986) Non-cholinergic neurotransmitter abnormalities in Alzheimer's disease. Br Med Bull 42:70–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahakian BJ (1988) Cholinergic drugs and human cognitive performance. In: Iversen L, Iversen SD, Snyder SH (eds) Handbook of Psychopharmacology, vol 20. Plenum Press, New York, pp 393–424

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, Heald A, Levy R, Philpot M, Robbins TW (1988) A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain 111:695–718

    Google Scholar 

  • Sitaram N, Weingartner H, Gillin JC (1978) Human serial learning: enhancement with arecoline and choline and impairment with scopolamine. Science 201:274–276

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith G (1988) Animal models of Alzheimer's disease: experimental cholinergic denervation. Brain Res Rev 13:103–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Weingartner H, Sitaram N, Gullin JC (1979) The role of the cholinergic nervous system in memory consolidation. Bull Psychonom Soc 13:9–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1984) Effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human rapid information processing performance. Psychopharmacology 82:147–150

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rupniak, N.M.J., Steventon, M.J., Field, M.J. et al. Comparison of the effects of four cholinomimetic agents on cognition in primates following disruption by scopolamine or by lists of objects. Psychopharmacology 99, 189–195 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00442806

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00442806

Key words

Navigation