Skip to main content
Log in

Phonological and lexical encoding processes in beginning readers: Effects of age and word characteristics

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The aim of this study was two-fold. First to find out whether the prelexical phonological route develops prior to the visual route or whether the direct visual route develops first in the process of learning to read Greek. Second, to see whether the subjects' chronological age deviation affects decisively the overall reading accuracy and the processes employed in word reading. For this an investigation was conducted involving two age groups of Primary-one class children regarded as normal readers and differing between them by 10 months. The subjects were tested in three word (and nonword) reading tasks in which psycholinguistic properties of words (such as grammatical class, imagery level, semantic content, regularity and frequency) had been manipulated. The main findings were: first that reading accuracy was not significantly affected by the main psycholinguistic properties of words. Second, that the chronological age factor is unlikely to influence decisively and consistently reading acquisition in the long term. Third, that Primary-one children although tending to use mainly phonological information in reading, nevertheless seem to have started making the transition towards the use of the direct visual route as well.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Babiniotis, G. 1980. Theoretical Linguistics. Athens (in Greek).

  • BackmanJ., BruckM., HebertM., and SeidenbergM. S. 1984. Spelling-sound correspondences in reading: Developmental aspects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 114–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • BaddeleyA. D., EllisN. C., MilesT. R., and LewisV. J. 1982. Developmental and acquired dyslexia: A comparison, Cognition, 11, 185–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • BarronR. W. 1980. Visual and phonological strategies in reading and spelling. In U.Frith (ed.), Cognitive Processes in Spelling. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • BradleyL. and BryantP. E. 1983. Categorising sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColtheartM. 1980. Reading, phonological recoding and deep dyslexia. In M.Coltheart, K. E.Patterson and J. C.Marshall (eds.), Deep Dyslexia. London: Routledge & Kegal Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColtheartM. 1983. Phonological awareness: A preschool precursor of success in reading. Nature, 301, 370.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColtheartM., DavelaarE., JonassonJ. T., and BesnerD. 1977. Access to the internal lexicon. In S.Dornic (ed.), Attention and Performance, vol. 6. Hillsdale: N.J. Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColtheartM., BesnerD., JonassonJ. T., and DavelaarE. 1979. Phonological recording in the lexical decision task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 489–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColtheartV., LaxonV., KeatingG. C., and PoolM. 1986. Direct access and phonological encoding processes in children's reading: Effects of word characteristics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 255–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColtheartV., LaxonV., and KeatingC. 1988. Effects of word imageability and age of acquisition on children's reading. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • DoctorE. A. and ColtheartM. 1980. Children's use of phonological encoding when reading for meaning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 195–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • EllisA. 1984. Reading, Writing and Dyslexia: A Cognitive Analysis. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • ForsterK. and ChambersS. 1973. Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 627–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • FrancisH. 1984. Children's knowledge of orthography in learning to read. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 8–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • FrithU. 1985. Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K.Patterson, J.Marshall and M.Coltheart (eds.), Surface Dyslexia: Neuropsychological and Cognitive Studies of Phonological Reading. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • GleitmanL. R. and RosinR. 1977. The structure and acquisition of reading I: Relations between orthographies and the structure of language. In A.Reber and D.Scarborough (eds.), Toward a Psychology of Reading. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • JormA. F. 1977. Effect of word imagery on reading performance as a function of reader ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 46–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • LibermanI. Y. 1971. Basic research in speech and lateralization of language: Some implications for reading disability. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 21, 71–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • LibermanI. Y. 1973. Segmentation of the spoken word and reading acquisition. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 23, 65–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • LibermanI. Y. and ShankweilerD. 1979. Speech, the alphabet and teaching to read. In L.Resnick and P.Weaver (eds.), Theory and Practice of Early Reading, Vol. 2. Hillsdale: Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LibermanI.Y., ShankweilerD., LibermanA. M., FowlerC., and FischerF. W. 1977. Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning reader. In A.Reder and D.Scarborough (eds.), Towards a Psychology of Reading. Hillsdale: Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LundbergI., OlofssonA., and WallS. 1980. Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • MarcelA. and PattersonK. 1978. Word recognition and production: Reciprocity of clinical and normal studies. In J.Requin (ed.), Attention and Performance. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MarschG., FriedmanM., WelchV., and DesbergP. 1981. A cognitive-developmental theory of reading acquisition. In G.Mackinnon and T. G.Waller (eds.), Reading Research: Advances in Theory and Practice. N.Y.: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MarshallJ. and NewcombeF. 1973. Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • MeyerD. E., SchvaneveldtR. W., and RuddyM. G. 1974. Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes in visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 2, 309–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • PorpodasC. D. 1986. The effect of articulatory suppression on Greek children's reading of Greek. Educational Psychology, 6, 115–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porpodas, C. D. 1989a. Learning to read and spell Greek in relation to age and phonemic awareness. A paper presented at the 1st Conference of Psychological Research, University of Thessaloniki, 19–21 May 1989 (In Greek).

  • PorpodasC. D. 1989b. The phonological factor in reading and spelling of Greek. In P. G.Aaron and R.Malatesha Joshi (eds.), Reading and Writing Disorders in Different Orthographic Systems, Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porpodas, C. D. 1989c. Linguistic Awareness, Verbal short-term memory and learning to read Greek. Research manuscript. University of Patras.

  • Porpodas, C. D. 1989d. Reading and Spelling at 5.6 or 6.6 years. Research Manuscript. University of Patras (In Greek).

  • RichardsonJ. T. E. 1976. The effects of stimulus attributes upon latency of world recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 315–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • RichardsonJ. T. E. 1984. Morphemic and phonemic factors in letter concellation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13, 351–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • RubensteinH., LewisS. S., and RubensteinM. A. 1971. Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 645–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • SeymourP. H. K. and ElderL. 1986. Reading without phonology. Cognitive Neuropsychology 3, 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • SeymourP. H. K. and MacGregorC. J. 1984. Developmental dyslexia: a cognitive experimental analysis of phonological, morphemic and visual impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 1–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • SeymourP. H. K. and PorpodasC. D. 1980. Lexical and non-lexical processing of spelling in dyslexia. In U.Frith (ed.), Cognitive Processes in Spelling. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ShalliceT. and WarringtonE. K. 1975. Word recognition in a phonemic dyslexic patient. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 187–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • ShankweilerD. and LibermanI. Y. 1972. Misreading: a search for causes. In J.Kavanagh and L.Mattingly (eds.), Language by Ear and by Eye: The Relationships between Speech and Reading, Cambridge Mass: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • TriantaphyllidisM. 1913. Our Orthography. Athens: Estia (In Greek).

    Google Scholar 

  • WatersG. S., BruckM., and SeidenbergM. 1985. Do children use similar processes to read and spell words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 511–530.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Requests for reprints should be sent to Dr. C. D. Porpodas, Section of Psychology, Department of Education, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Patras, 26110 Patras, Greece.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Porpodas, C.D., Pantelis, S.N. & Hantziou, E. Phonological and lexical encoding processes in beginning readers: Effects of age and word characteristics. Read Writ 2, 197–208 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00257971

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00257971

Keywords

Navigation