Skip to main content
Log in

Common principle of guidance by echolocation and vision

  • Published:
Journal of Comparative Physiology A Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

  1. 1.

    Using echolocation, bats move as gracefully as birds through the cluttered environment, suggesting common principles of optic and acoustic guidance. We tested the idea by analysing braking control of bats (Macroderma gigas) flying through a narrow aperture with eyes covered and uncovered.

  2. 2.

    Though braking control would seem to require rapid detection of distance and velocity and computation of deceleration, simpler control is possible using the tau function of any sensory variable S that is a power function of distance to aperture. Tau function of S is τ(S) = S/S (the dot means time derivative). Controlled braking is achievable by keeping τ(S) constant.

  3. 3.

    Previous experiments indicated the τ(S) constant procedure is followed by humans and birds in visually controlling braking. Analysis of the bats' flight trajectories indicated they too followed the braking procedure using echolocation.

  4. 4.

    The tau function of echo-delay or of echo-intensity or of angle subtended by directions of echoes from two points on the approach surface could be used to control braking. Aperture size was modulated during flight on some trials in an attempt to test between these possibilities, but the results were inconclusive.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bastian J (1986) Electrolocation: behavior, anatomy and physiology. In: Bullock TH, Heiligenberg W (eds) Electroreception. Wiley, New York, pp 577–612

    Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma RJ, van Wieringen PWC (1990) Timing an attacking for hand drive in table tennis. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 16:21–29

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock TH, Heiligenberg W (eds) (1986) Electroreception. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin DR (1958) Listening in the dark. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin DR, Novick A, Kornfield M (1958) The sensitivity of echolocation in the fruit bat Rousettus. Biol Bull 115:107–113

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinnell AD, Griffin DR (1958) The sensitivity of echolocation in bats. Biol Bull 114:10–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Gustafson Y, Schnitzler HU (1979) Echolocation and obstacle avoidance in the hipposiderid bat Asellia tridens. J Comp Physiol 131:161–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Kick SA, Simmons JA (1984) Automatic gain control in the bat's sonar receiver and the neuroethology of echolocation. J Neurosci 4:2725–2737

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacquaniti F, Maiolo C (1989) The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching. J Neurosci 9:134–148

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN (1976) A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception 5:437–459

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN (1980) The optic flow field: The foundation of vision. Phil Trans R Soc London B 290:169–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN (1991) Optic and acoustic constraints for action. In: Neisser U (ed) Ecological and interpersonal knowledge of the self. University Press, Cambridge, in press

    Chapter  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Reddish PE (1981) Plummeting gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics. Nature 293:293–294

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Young DS (1986) Gearing action to the environment. In: Heuer H, Fromm C (eds) Generation and modulation of action patterns. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 217–230

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Lishman JR, Thomson JA (1982) Visual regulation of gait in long jumping. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 8:448–459

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Young DS, Reddish PE, Lough S, Clayton TMH (1983) Visual timing in hitting an accelerating ball. Q J Exp Psychol 35A:333–346

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Reddish PE, Rand DT (1991) Aerial docking by hummingbirds. Naturwissenschaften 78:526–527

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore BCJ (1989) An introduction to the psychology of hearing. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuweiler G, Möhres FP (1967) Die Rolle des Ortsgedächtnisses bei der Orientierung der Grossblatt-Fledermaus Megaderma lyra. Z Vergl Physiol 57:147–171

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblum LD, Carello C, Pastore RE (1987) Relative effectiveness of three stimulus variables for locating a moving sound source. Perception 16:175–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, Whiting HTA, Bootsma RJ (1991) Grasping “tau”. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 17:315–322

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Detwiler ML (1979) Information used in judging impending collision. Perception 8:647–658

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Oldak R (1990) Accuracy of judging time-to-arrival: Effects of modality, trajectory and gender. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 16:303–316

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnitzler HU (1973) Control of Doppler shift compensation in the greater horseshoe bat Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. J Comp Physiol 82:79–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnitzler HU, Henson OW (1980) Performance of airborne animal sonar systems: I Microchiroptera. In: Busnel RG, Fish JF (eds) Animal sonar systems. Plenum, New York, pp 109–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidaway B, McNitt-Gray J, Davis G (1989) Visual timing of muscle preactivation in preparation for landing. Ecol Psychol 1:253–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA (1989) A view of the world through the bat's ear: the formation of acoustic images in echolocation. Cognition 33:155–199

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA, Ferragamo M, Moss CF, Stevenson SB, Altes RA (1990) Discrimination of jittered sonar echoes by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus: the shape of the target images in echolocation. J Comp Physiol A 167:589–616

    Google Scholar 

  • Suga N (1984) Neural mechanisms of complex-sound processing for echolocation. Trends Neurosci 7:20–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Suga N (1988) Parallel-hierarchical processing of biosonar information in the mustached bat. In: Nachtigall PE, Moore PWB (eds) Animal sonar: processes and performance. Plenum Press, New York, pp 149–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd JT (1981) Visual information about moving objects. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 7:795–810

    Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR (1990) Perceptual information for the timing of interceptive action. Perception 19:223–239

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner H (1982) Flow-field variables trigger landing in flies. Nature 297:147–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH, Young DS, Lee DN (1986) Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 12:259–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster FA (1967) Some acoustical differences between bats and men. In: Dufton R (ed) International conference on sensory devices for the blind. St Dunstan's, London, pp 63–88

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lee, D.N., van der Weel, F.R.(., Hitchcock, T. et al. Common principle of guidance by echolocation and vision. J Comp Physiol A 171, 563–571 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00194105

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00194105

Key words

Navigation