Skip to main content

From Causality to Catalysis in the Social Sciences

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Social Philosophy of Science for the Social Sciences

Part of the book series: Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences ((THHSS))

Abstract

Our knowledge is trapped in the discourse about causality. This trap is set by the common language notions of something causing something else and its penetration into scientific domains. The crucial feature of the phenomena in the social sciences is the flexibility for intentional coordination of conditions of personal and collective cultures with social representations which is a feature absent at the lower levels of catalysis. This is made possible for the use of sign systems at various levels—personal, communal, societal, economic, and political. We can look at human phenomena as semiotically catalyzed. Social sciences introduce a new demand for philosophy of science—to account for the agency of purposeful actors and their co(unter)-actions in any generalized scheme of catalytic processes. This demand is an opportunity that may lead all social sciences toward understanding the dramatic realities of the human condition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Pseudo-empiricism is the research practice in which empirical investigation is undertaken to prove some propositions that already are given in the normative system shared by the researcher and researches. The result is camouflaging the normative implicit knowledge by its “empirical discovery” in the data.

  2. 2.

    Since every straight line can be viewed as a part of an infinitely large circle—where both ends of the straight-looking line eventually meet.

  3. 3.

    The psychology and sociology of religious feelings were an honorable research topic in the social sciences in its history, all through to the 1920s. After that it declined in favor of comparative perspective—comparing various religious denominations with one another, without looking into the functions of particular religious rituals in human lives.

  4. 4.

    Painters painted mountains without leaving their studios. It was only on rare occasions that they were asked to paint from the nature itself.

  5. 5.

    Precisely similar role of catalysts is played by fiction—in the reading of a novel or a short story, specific features of the text enable, but do not cause, the reader to arrive at new experiences, including life philosophical hyper-generalizations (cf Valsiner, 2015; Vygotsky, 1971).

References

  • Brinkmann, S. (2010). Guilt in a fluid culture? A view from positioning theory. Culture & Psychology, 16(2), 253–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2016). Psychology as a normative science. In J. Valsiner, G. Marsico, N. Chaudhary, T. Sato, & V. Dazzani (Eds.), Psychology as the science of human being (pp. 3–16). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2019). Normativity in psychology and the social sciences: Questions of universality. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Social philosophy of science for the social sciences. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabell, K. R., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2014). The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality. Vol, 11 of Advances of Theoretical Psychology. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carus, C. G. (2002/1831). Nine letters on landscape painting. Los Angeles, CA: Getty Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaudhary, N., Hviid, P., Marsico, G., & Villadsen, J. W. (Eds.). (2017). Resistance in everyday life: Constructing cultural experiences. Singapore: Springer Nature.

    Google Scholar 

  • Da Silva, M. (2014). Semiotic catalysts’ activators: Na early semiotic mediation in the construction of personal synthesis. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 251–268). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • de Mattos, E. (2018). Anticipatory recognition: Creating a cycle of flexible meanings in intergenerational relations. In I. Albert, E. Abbey, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Trans-generational family relations: Investigating ambivalences (pp. 73–98). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (1991). From tools to theories: A heuristic of discovery in cognitive psychology. Psychological Review, 98, 254–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodyear, W. H. (1891). The grammar of the lotus. London, UK: Sampson Low, Marston & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kadianaki, I., & Zittoun, T. (2014). Catalysts and regulators of psychological change in the context of immigration ruptures. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 191–207). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Klempe, S. H. (Ed.). (2016). Cultural psychology of music. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, A. (2014). Cause and catalyst: A differentiation. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 33–70). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kull, K. (2014). Catalysis and scaffolding in semiosis. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 111–121). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Malnes, R. (2019). Explanation: Guidance for social scientists. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Social philosophy of science for the social sciences. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mascolo, M. (2017). The transformation of a white supremacist: A dialectical-developmental analysis. Qualitative Psychology, 4(3), 223–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (1999). Measurement in psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mittasch, A. (1938). Katalyse und Determinismus. Berlin, Germany: Julius Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Musaeus, P., & Brinkmann, S. (2011). The semiosis of family conflict: A case study of home-based psychotherapy. Culture & Psychology, 17(1), 47–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nedergaard, J. I., Valsiner, J., & Marsico, G. (2015). “I am not that kind of…”: Personal relating with social borders. In B. Wagoner, N. Chaudhary, & P. Hviid (Eds.), Integrating experiences: Body and mind moving between contexts (pp. 245–263). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puche Navarro, R. (Ed.). (2009). Es la mente no lineal? Cali, Colombia: Programa editorial Universidad del Valle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (1995). Psychologic: Common sense and the pseudoempirical. In J. A. Smith, R. HarrĂ©, & L. van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking psychology (pp. 196–206). London, UK: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strand, R. (2019). Vitenskapsteori—What, how and why? In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Social philosophy of science for the social sciences. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L. (2014). Beyond the self and the environment: The psychological horizon. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 223–237). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2014). A structural systemic theory of causality and catalysis. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 271–292). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1996). Devadasi temple dancers and cultural construction of persons-in-society. In M. K. Raha (Ed.), Dimensions of human society and culture (pp. 443–476). New Delhi, India: Gyan Publishing House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1999). I create you to control me: A glimpse into basic processes of semiotic mediation. Human Development, 42, 26–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2014). Breaking the arrows of causality: The idea of catalysis in its making. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 17–32). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2015). The place for synthesis: Vygotsky’s analysis of affective generalization. History of the Human Sciences, 28(2), 93–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2019). Ornamented lives. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Brinkmann, S. (2016). Beyond the “variables”: Developing metalanguage in psychology. In S. H. Klempe & R. Smith (Eds.), Centrality of history for theory construction in psychology (pp. 75–90). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Rudolph, L. (2012). Who shall survive? Psychology that replaces quantification with qualitative mathematics. In E. Abbey & S. Surgan (Eds.), Emerging methods in psychology (pp. 121–140). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Sato, T. (2006). Historically Structured Sampling (HSS): How can psychology’s methodology become tuned in to the reality of the historical nature of cultural psychology? In J. Straub, D. Weidemann, C. Kölbl, & B. Zielke (Eds.), Pursuit of meaning (pp. 215–251). Bielefeld, Germany: Transcript.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1971). Psychology of art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B. (2014). A systemic approach to cultural diffusion and reproduction. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality (pp. 125–147). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B., & Valsiner, J. (2005). Rating tasks in psychology: from static ontology to dialogical synthesis of meaning. In A. GĂĽlerce, A. Hofmeister, I. Staeuble, G. Saunders and J. Kaye (Eds.), Contemporary theorizing in psychology: Global perspectives (pp. 197–213). Toronto: Captus Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Wikan, U. (2002). Generous betrayal: Politics of culture in the new Europe. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

The suggestions on an earlier version of this chapter by Svend Brinkmann are gratefully acknowledged.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Valsiner, J. (2019). From Causality to Catalysis in the Social Sciences. In: Valsiner, J. (eds) Social Philosophy of Science for the Social Sciences. Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-33099-6_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics