Skip to main content
Log in

Implicit Cross-Community Biases Revisited: Evidence for Ingroup Favoritism in the Absence of Outgroup Derogation in Northern Ireland

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite their application in virtually every area of psychological science, indirect procedures have rarely been used to study how Catholic and Protestants automatically respond to one another in Northern Ireland. What little evidence that does exist suggests that automatic ingroup favoritism occurs alongside outgroup derogation. That is, Catholics and Protestants automatically evaluate ingroup members more positively than outgroup members, and also evaluate outgroup members more negatively than ingroup members. The current study addresses a methodological limitation in this early work and provides the first (non-relativistic) assessment of intergroup relational responding in a post-conflict setting using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). Contrary to earlier findings, participants displayed evidence of ingroup favoritism in the absence of outgroup derogation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Let us be clear from the outset. Stating that IRAP effects are non-relative is not the same as saying that they are a-contextual. Non-relative denotes that the effect itself is calculated in a way that is independent from other trial types (i.e., what we say about the Catholics-Good trial type is inferred from the speed with which people affirm versus reject the Catholic-Good relation, and does not depend on how quickly they responded to “Catholics” and negative terms or “Protestants” and positive or negative terms). Nevertheless, this does not mean non-relativistic trial-type effects are a-contextual. In other words, the relational response on any given trial, and thus the effects calculated from those responses, could be moderated by contextual variables that are part of the IRAP or the wider context in which the IRAP is embedded (for more on non-relative vs. a-contextual, see Hussey et al., 2016). We will return to this issue in greater detail later on in the General Discussion.

  2. The terms “ingroup favoritism” and “outgroup derogation” are often used in the social psychological literature to refer to a set of mental concepts and processes (see Dasgupta, 2004). Although we will use those same terms in this paper, we make no appeals to, or assumptions about, those mental mechanisms. Instead, we simply use these terms to orient the reader towards specific patterns of behavior (relational responses) that are emitted by members of different groups (Catholics and Protestants) in the presence of certain stimuli (ingroup vs. outgroup exemplars and trait descriptions).

References

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Hussey, I., & Luciano, C. (2016). Relational frame theory: Finding its historical and philosophical roots and reflecting upon its future development: An introduction to part II. In R. D. Zettle, S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & A. Biglan (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of contextual behavioral science (pp. 117–128). West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Relational Elaboration and Coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record, 60, 527–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bast, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015). Developing an individualized Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a potential measure of self-forgiveness related to negative and positive behavior. The Psychological Record, 65, 717–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, E., & Darby, J. (1998). The conflict in Northern Ireland: causes, consequences, andcontrols. American Psychologist, 53(7), 754–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N. (2004). Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioralmanifestations. Social Justice Research, 17, 143–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Heider, N., Spruyt, A., Roets, A., & Hughes, S. (2015). The relational respondingtask: toward a new implicit measure of beliefs. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 319. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., Dymond, S., Rehfeldt, R. A., Roche, B., & Zlomke, K. R. (2003). Terrorism and relational frame theory. Behavior and Social Issues, 12, 129–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, C. E., Kramer, S., Sain, T., Swiatek, R., Kohn, K., & Murphy, M. (2015). Exploring the reliability and convergent validity of implicit propositional evaluations of race. Behavior and Social Issues, 24, 68–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, C. E., Seymour, K. H., & Habib, R. (2016). Testing the IRAP: exploring the reliability and fakability of an idiographic approach to interpersonal attitudes. The Psychological Record, 66, 153–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, L., Cochrane, A., & McHugh, L. (2015). Exploring attitudes towards gender and science: the advantages of an IRAP approach versus the IAT. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 121–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, F., Barnes-Holmes, D., Hussey, I., & Graddy, J. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the implicit relational assessment procedure: the impact of three types of introductory rules. The Psychological Record, 66, 309–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 283–310). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. an improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M., Lolliot, S., Swart, H., Myers, E., Voci, A., Al Ramiah, A., & Cairns, E. (2014). Intergroup contact and intergroup conflict. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 20, 39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, J., Campbell, A., Lolliot, S., Hewstone, M., & Gallagher, T. (2013). Inter-group contact at school and social attitudes: evidence from Northern Ireland. Oxford Review of Education, 39, 761–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S., Hussey, I., Corrigan, B., Jolie, K., Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Faking revisited: exerting strategic control over performance on the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 632–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., Mhaoileoin, D. N., Barnes-Holmes, D., Ohtsuki, T., Kishita, N., Hughes, S., & Murphy, C. (2016). The IRAP is nonrelative but not a-contextual: changes to the contrast category influence men’s dehumanization of women. The Psychological Record, 66, 291–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., Thompson, M., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2015). Interpreting and inverting with less cursing: a guide to interpreting IRAP data. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 157–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S., Rogge, R. D., & Reis, H. T. (2010). Assessing the seeds of relationship decay using implicit evaluations to detect the early stages of disillusionment. Psychological Science, 21, 857–864.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: the role of relational contextual cues versus relational coherence indicators as response options. The Psychological Record, 66, 395–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAlister, S., Scraton, P., & Haydon, D. (2009). Childhood in transition. Experiencing marginalisation and conflict in Northern Ireland. Queen’s University Belfast, Save the Children, The Prince’s Trust: Belfast.

  • McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKittrick, D., Kelters, S., Feeney, B., Thornton, C., & McVea, D. (2007). Lost lives: The stories of the men, women and children who died as a result of the Northern Ireland troubles. Edinburgh: Mainstream Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2012). Developing an implicit measure of disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: examining the role of implicit disgust propensity and sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 922–930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 511–517.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Northern Ireland Council for Integrated Education. (2014). Annual report. Retrieved January 3, 2014, from http://www.nicie.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/11/NICIE-Annual-Report-13-14-web.pdf.

  • Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Shea, B., Watson, D. G., & Brown, G. D. A. (2016). Measuring implicit attitudes: a positive framing bias flaw in the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). Psychological Assessment, 28, 158–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rooth, D. O. (2010). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: real world evidence. Labour Economics, 17, 523–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shirlow, P., & Murtagh, B. (2006). Belfast: Segregation, violence and the city. London: Pluto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J. B., Cairns, E., Marinetti, C., Geddes, L., & Parkinson, B. (2008). Postconflict reconciliation: intergroup forgiveness and implicit biases in Northern Ireland. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 303–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timmins, L., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Cullen, C. (2016). Measuring implicit sexual response biases to nude male and female pictures in androphilic and gynephilic men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 829–841.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, R. N., Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J., & Cairns, E. (2013). Contact between Catholic and Protestant schoolchildren in Northern Ireland. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 216–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The second author was supported by an Odysseus (Type 1) award from the Flanders Science Foundation (FWO) during preparation of this article. Electronic mail should be sent to sean.hughes@ugent.be. This paper is dedicated the memory of Ed Cairns, who inspired and facilitated this particular line of research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean Hughes.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded by a postgraduate scholarship to the first author from the Irish Research Council for Science, Engineering, and Technology (IRCSET).

Conflict of Interest

The authors (SH, DBH, and SS) declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the host institution and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Studies with Animals

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendices

Appendix A

Fig. 1
figure 1

Examples of the four trial types used in the IRAP. The label stimuli (e.g., “Catholics” and “Protestants”), target stimuli (e.g., “friendly” and “violent”) and relational response options (“True” and “False”) are indicated

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean D- IRAP scores as a function of Community Background (Catholic vs. Protestant). A positive value indicates a positivity bias while a negative score indicates a negativity bias towards a given group. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean self-reported evaluative responses as a function of Community Background (Catholic vs. Protestant). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals

Appendix B

D-IRAP scores can be calculated in the following way: (1) discard response-latency data from practice blocks and only use test blocks data; (2) eliminate latencies above 10,000 ms from the data set; (3) remove all data for a participant if he or she produces more than 10% of test-block trials with latencies less than 300 ms; (4) compute 12 standard deviations for the four trial types: four from the response latencies from Test Blocks 1 and 2, four from the latencies from Test Blocks 3 and 4, and four from Test Blocks 5 and 6; (5) calculate the mean latencies for the four trial types in each test block (resulting in 24 mean latencies in total); (6) calculate difference scores for each of the four trial types for each pair of test blocks by subtracting the mean latency of the Rule A block from the mean latency of the corresponding Rule B block; (7) divide each difference score by its corresponding standard deviation (see step 4). This yields 12 D-IRAP scores, one score for each trial type for each pair of test blocks. Finally, (8) calculate four overall trial type scores by averaging the scores for each trial type across the three pairs of test blocks. Note that these four trial-type scores can be collapsed into an overall D-IRAP score if the researcher so chooses.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hughes, S., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Smyth, S. Implicit Cross-Community Biases Revisited: Evidence for Ingroup Favoritism in the Absence of Outgroup Derogation in Northern Ireland. Psychol Rec 67, 97–107 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0210-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0210-3

Keywords

Navigation