The Psychological Record

, Volume 67, Issue 1, pp 97–107 | Cite as

Implicit Cross-Community Biases Revisited: Evidence for Ingroup Favoritism in the Absence of Outgroup Derogation in Northern Ireland

Original Article

Abstract

Despite their application in virtually every area of psychological science, indirect procedures have rarely been used to study how Catholic and Protestants automatically respond to one another in Northern Ireland. What little evidence that does exist suggests that automatic ingroup favoritism occurs alongside outgroup derogation. That is, Catholics and Protestants automatically evaluate ingroup members more positively than outgroup members, and also evaluate outgroup members more negatively than ingroup members. The current study addresses a methodological limitation in this early work and provides the first (non-relativistic) assessment of intergroup relational responding in a post-conflict setting using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). Contrary to earlier findings, participants displayed evidence of ingroup favoritism in the absence of outgroup derogation.

Keywords

Implicit cognition Northern Ireland IRAP 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The second author was supported by an Odysseus (Type 1) award from the Flanders Science Foundation (FWO) during preparation of this article. Electronic mail should be sent to sean.hughes@ugent.be. This paper is dedicated the memory of Ed Cairns, who inspired and facilitated this particular line of research.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Funding

This study was funded by a postgraduate scholarship to the first author from the Irish Research Council for Science, Engineering, and Technology (IRCSET).

Conflict of Interest

The authors (SH, DBH, and SS) declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the host institution and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Studies with Animals

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Hussey, I., & Luciano, C. (2016). Relational frame theory: Finding its historical and philosophical roots and reflecting upon its future development: An introduction to part II. In R. D. Zettle, S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & A. Biglan (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of contextual behavioral science (pp. 117–128). West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
  2. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Relational Elaboration and Coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record, 60, 527–542.Google Scholar
  3. Bast, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015). Developing an individualized Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a potential measure of self-forgiveness related to negative and positive behavior. The Psychological Record, 65, 717–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cairns, E., & Darby, J. (1998). The conflict in Northern Ireland: causes, consequences, andcontrols. American Psychologist, 53(7), 754–760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dasgupta, N. (2004). Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioralmanifestations. Social Justice Research, 17, 143–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. De Houwer, J., Heider, N., Spruyt, A., Roets, A., & Hughes, S. (2015). The relational respondingtask: toward a new implicit measure of beliefs. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 319. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00319.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Dixon, M. R., Dymond, S., Rehfeldt, R. A., Roche, B., & Zlomke, K. R. (2003). Terrorism and relational frame theory. Behavior and Social Issues, 12, 129–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Drake, C. E., Kramer, S., Sain, T., Swiatek, R., Kohn, K., & Murphy, M. (2015). Exploring the reliability and convergent validity of implicit propositional evaluations of race. Behavior and Social Issues, 24, 68–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Drake, C. E., Seymour, K. H., & Habib, R. (2016). Testing the IRAP: exploring the reliability and fakability of an idiographic approach to interpersonal attitudes. The Psychological Record, 66, 153–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Farrell, L., Cochrane, A., & McHugh, L. (2015). Exploring attitudes towards gender and science: the advantages of an IRAP approach versus the IAT. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 121–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Finn, F., Barnes-Holmes, D., Hussey, I., & Graddy, J. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the implicit relational assessment procedure: the impact of three types of introductory rules. The Psychological Record, 66, 309–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 283–310). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  14. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. an improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hewstone, M., Lolliot, S., Swart, H., Myers, E., Voci, A., Al Ramiah, A., & Cairns, E. (2014). Intergroup contact and intergroup conflict. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 20, 39–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hughes, J., Campbell, A., Lolliot, S., Hewstone, M., & Gallagher, T. (2013). Inter-group contact at school and social attitudes: evidence from Northern Ireland. Oxford Review of Education, 39, 761–779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hughes, S., Hussey, I., Corrigan, B., Jolie, K., Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Faking revisited: exerting strategic control over performance on the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 632–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hussey, I., Mhaoileoin, D. N., Barnes-Holmes, D., Ohtsuki, T., Kishita, N., Hughes, S., & Murphy, C. (2016). The IRAP is nonrelative but not a-contextual: changes to the contrast category influence men’s dehumanization of women. The Psychological Record, 66, 291–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hussey, I., Thompson, M., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2015). Interpreting and inverting with less cursing: a guide to interpreting IRAP data. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 157–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lee, S., Rogge, R. D., & Reis, H. T. (2010). Assessing the seeds of relationship decay using implicit evaluations to detect the early stages of disillusionment. Psychological Science, 21, 857–864.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Maloney, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: the role of relational contextual cues versus relational coherence indicators as response options. The Psychological Record, 66, 395–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McAlister, S., Scraton, P., & Haydon, D. (2009). Childhood in transition. Experiencing marginalisation and conflict in Northern Ireland. Queen’s University Belfast, Save the Children, The Prince’s Trust: Belfast.Google Scholar
  25. McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. McKittrick, D., Kelters, S., Feeney, B., Thornton, C., & McVea, D. (2007). Lost lives: The stories of the men, women and children who died as a result of the Northern Ireland troubles. Edinburgh: Mainstream Publishing.Google Scholar
  27. Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2012). Developing an implicit measure of disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: examining the role of implicit disgust propensity and sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 922–930.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Nisbett, R., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 511–517.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Northern Ireland Council for Integrated Education. (2014). Annual report. Retrieved January 3, 2014, from http://www.nicie.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/11/NICIE-Annual-Report-13-14-web.pdf.
  31. Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. O'Shea, B., Watson, D. G., & Brown, G. D. A. (2016). Measuring implicit attitudes: a positive framing bias flaw in the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). Psychological Assessment, 28, 158–170.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Rooth, D. O. (2010). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: real world evidence. Labour Economics, 17, 523–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shirlow, P., & Murtagh, B. (2006). Belfast: Segregation, violence and the city. London: Pluto Press.Google Scholar
  35. Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J. B., Cairns, E., Marinetti, C., Geddes, L., & Parkinson, B. (2008). Postconflict reconciliation: intergroup forgiveness and implicit biases in Northern Ireland. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 303–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Timmins, L., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Cullen, C. (2016). Measuring implicit sexual response biases to nude male and female pictures in androphilic and gynephilic men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 829–841.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Turner, R. N., Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J., & Cairns, E. (2013). Contact between Catholic and Protestant schoolchildren in Northern Ireland. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 216–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ghent UniversityGhentBelgium
  2. 2.Dublin College UniversityDublinIreland

Personalised recommendations