Skip to main content

Spanish se as a High and Low Verbalizer

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Unraveling the complexity of SE

Part of the book series: Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory ((SNLT,volume 99))

  • 234 Accesses

Abstract

The clitic se in Spanish appears in a number of different constructions, including the reflexive, anticausative and antipassive. It even appears with certain unergative verbs. So far, a unified explanation for the polyfunctionality of se has remained elusive. In this paper, I propose that se functions as a verbalizer (see also Kallulli D, (Non-)canonical passives and reflexives. In Alexiadou A, Schäfer F. Non-canonical passives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 337–358, 2013) in two different domains. When it merges low, it functions to verbalize a nominal element and introduces an external argument. In this instance we see the clitic se in an unergative structure. In the second domain, the clitic merges higher and takes as its complement a Predicate Phrase (PredP) that contains in its specifier the derived position of the internal argument. This PredP semantically does not denote a predicate of events but a predicate of individuals. In this higher position, the clitic (re)verbalizes the structure, taking the predicate of individuals and creating a predicate of events. By creating a predicate of events, the verb can interact with tense and other verbal functional elements in the clause. The polyfunctionality of the clitic se results because of its verbalizing function.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A reviewer reminds me that it is possible for the argument position to contain a reflexive pronoun along with the clitic, as in the following French sentence from Labelle (2008).

    The reviewer suggests that this may pose a problem for the analysis advocated here. However, in Labelle’s (2008) analysis, this overt reflexive pronoun does not saturate the argument position of the verb and considers the possibility that this pronoun “translates as a formula containing a variable” (847). If this pronoun does translate as a variable, then it is another way for the internal argument of the [V refl V ] complex to have its internal argument saturated and meet the bound variable requirement of the internal argument imposed by refl.

  2. 2.

    Kempchinsky (2004) notes that ‘verbs of grooming’ in Spanish behave differently with respect to clitic doubling and omission of se under causative verbs such as hacer; thus it may be the case that these verbs, too, in Spanish also have a different lexical specification.

  3. 3.

    The optional dative clitic which agrees with the indirect object appears in the head of Appl, following Cuervo (2003, 2014). I propose that this clitic does not appear in the reflexive case because PRO lacks features for agreement.

  4. 4.

    Although this construction is often termed the antipassive, this construction is not completely parallel to canonical antipassive structures seen in ergative languages such as Chukchi or Inuit. (For a recent discussion of the antipassive in Inuit, see Spreng 2012.) Like the canonical antipassive, we see an alternation where, with special morphology on the verb, the external argument remains while the internal argument appears as an oblique or is missing entirely. However, unlike these ergative languages, where the antipassive construction is productive, this construction in Spanish is limited to a few lexical items, as noted by Armstrong (2011).

  5. 5.

    A reviewer notes that the reflexive/non-reflexive forms are more related in meaning than what the translations above suggest. The verb despedir means ‘fire’ in the sense of ‘dismiss from a job’. If you dismiss someone, you send them away, which relates to the sense of despedirse de in that when you say goodbye to someone, you are sending them away. We can see the same relatedness with the verb deshacer ‘to take apart’ and its reflexive counterpart. When something is taken apart, each elements is getting away from another element. In the reflexive deshacerse de, the elements are getting away from you or your belongings, which relates to the sense of ‘get rid of’. Even though these two verbs have related senses, they do not have identical senses in every case. We expect as the meanings drift we would get two separate lexical items.

  6. 6.

    A reviewer worries that this analysis is merely descriptively adequate rather than explanatorily adequate, in that the two argument structures are simply listed. However, this ‘antipassive’ construction is lexically limited, so there must be something special with respect to these verbs. My goal here is to explain the appearance of the se clitic and link the appearance of the clitic in this construction with its appearance in other constructions. The explanatory link between the antipassive and the reflexive is that in both cases, the internal argument is introduced in a low position and in a different position than that of the transitive.

  7. 7.

    The Russian construction in (6) above has been likened to the antipassive, in that the agent argument remains while the internal argument is missing when–sja is present. This construction is also termed the ‘absolutive’, and only appears with a limited set of verbs (such as bodat’(sja) ‘to butt’, ljagat’(sja) ‘to kick’). We see the same use of the tko/tku suffix in Chukchi (Kozinsky, Nedjalkov and Polinskaja 1988).

    In this case, we can consider that there are two verbs ‘bite’, one which is just a predicate of events and one which is a relation between an event and an entity. However, unlike in the reflexive, the entity argument is not a bound variable. Syntactically, it will be represented as arbitrary PRO.

    Interestingly, this construction does not occur in Spanish.

  8. 8.

    A similar situation occurs in Polish (Szczesniak 2006).

References

  • Acedo-Matellán, Victor, and Jaume Mateu. 2015. From syntax to roots: A syntactic approach to root interpretation. In The roots of syntax, the syntax of roots, ed. Artemis Alexiadou, Hagit Borer, and Florian Schaefer, 14–32. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis. 2014. Roots don’t take complements. Theoretical Linguistics 40: 287–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis and Florian Schäfer. 2011. There-insertion: an unaccusative mismatch at the syntax-semantics interface. Online Proceedings of WCCFL 28. https://sites.google.com/site/wccfl28pro/alexiadou-schaefer.

  • Alexiadou, Artemis, Florian Schäfer, and Giorgos Spathas. 2013. Delimiting voice in Germanic: On object drop and naturally reflexive verbs. In Proceedings of 44th meeting of the North East linguistic society, ed. Jyoti Iyer and Leland Kusmer, 1–14. Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistics Students Association, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis, Elena Anagnostopoulou, and Florian Schäfer. 2015. Exernal arguments in transitivity alternations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, Grant. 2011. Two classes of transitive verbs: Evidence from Spanish. PhD diss., Georgetown University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark. 1988. Incorporation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basilico, David. 1998. Object positions and predication forms. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 16: 541–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2008. Particle verbs and benefactive double object constructions: High and low attachments. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26: 731–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. The se clitic and its relationship to paths. Probus 22: 271–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, Hagit. 2005. Structuring sense Vol II: The normal course of events. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2013. Structuring sense Vol III: Taking form. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, John. 2010. Arguments as relations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuervo, Maria Cristina. 2003. Datives at large. PhD diss., MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2014. Alternation unaccusatives and the distribution of roots. Lingua 141: 48–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dekker, Paul. 1993. Existential Disclosure. Linguistics and Philosophy 16: 561–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, David. 2004. Unaccusative syntax and verbal alternations. In The Unaccusativity puzzle, ed. A. Alexiadou, E. Anagnostopolou, and M. Everaert, 137–158. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, Kay, and Samuel Jay Keyser. 2002. Prolegomenon to a theory of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, Heidi, and Rafaella Folli. 2005. Flavors of V: Consuming results in Italian and English. In Aspectual inquiries, ed. Roumyana Slabakova and Paula Kempchinsky, 95–120. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, Kyle. 1991. Object positions. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 9: 577–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kallulli, Dalina. 2013. (Non-)canonical passives and reflexives. In Non-canonical passives, ed. Artemis Alexiadou and Florian Schäfer, 337–358. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kempchinsky, Paula. 2004. Romance SE as an aspectual element. In Contemporary approaches to romance linguistics, ed. J. Auger et al., 239–256. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, Robert. 1992. Deriving Island constraints from the principles of predication. In Island constraints: Theory, acquisition and processing, ed. Helen Goodluck and Michael Rochemont, 223–258. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Koontz-Garboden, Andrew. 2009. Anticausativization. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 27: 77–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozinsky, Isaac, Vladimir P. Nedjalkov, and Marija S. Polinskaja. 1988. Antipassive in Chukchee: Oblique object, object incorporation, zero object. In Passive and voice, ed. Masayoshi Shibatani, 651–707. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. Severing the external argument from its verb. In Phrase structure and the lexicon, ed. Johan Rooryck and Laurie Zaring, 109–137. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuroda, S.-Y. 1972. The categorical and the thetic judgement. Foundations of Language 9: 153–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labelle, Marie. 2008. The French reflexive and reciprocal se. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26: 833–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, Beth. 1993. English verb classes and alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohndahl Terje. 2012. Without specifiers: Phrase structure and events. PhD diss., University of Maryland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohndahl, Terje. 2014. Phrase structure and argument structure: A case study of the syntax-semantics interface. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 1984. On the nature of grammatical relations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masullo, Pascual José. 1992. Antipassive constructions in Spanish. In Romance languages and modern linguistic theory, ed. Paul Hirschbühler and E.F.K. Koerner, 175–194. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pylkkänen, Liina. 2008. Introducing arguments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, Gillian. 2008. Verb meaning and the lexicon: A first phase syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Say, Sergei. 2005. Antipassive –sja verbs in Russian: Between inflection and derivation. In Morphology and its demarcations, ed. Wolfgang U. Dressler, Dieter Kastovsky, Oskar E. Pfeiffer, and Franz Rainer, 253–276. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schäfer, Florian. 2008. The syntax of (anti-)causatives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Spreng, Bettina. 2012. Viewpoint aspect in inuktitut. PhD diss., University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svenonius, Peter. 2002. Icelandic case and the structure of events. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 5: 197–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tatevosov, Sergei. 2013. Event structure of the anticausative and unaccusative in Russian. Paper presented at IATL 29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis, Lisa de Mena. 2010. Inner aspect: The articulation of VP. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltschko, Martina. 2006. On ‘Ergativity’ in Halkomelem Salish. In Ergativity: Emerging issues, ed. Alana Johns, Diane Massam, and Juvenal Ndayiragije, 197–228. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, Jim. 2012. Icelandic morphosyntax and argument structure. PhD diss., NYU.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank the audience at the Workshop on se/si for their questions and suggestions as well as the two anonymous reviewers for further input on this paper. I especially thank Grant Armstrong and Jonathan MacDonald for organizing and hosting the conference as well as for their editorial input. Of course, all errors are my responsibility.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Basilico .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Basilico, D. (2021). Spanish se as a High and Low Verbalizer. In: Armstrong, G., MacDonald, J.E. (eds) Unraveling the complexity of SE. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, vol 99. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-57004-0_13

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics