Abstract
In a group of animals, interests and needs among group members can be very different. To benefit from group-living, individuals must therefore coordinate each other’s activities when they perform collective actions. Collective decision-making is a key mechanism during coordination, and it allows all members of a social group to reach a consensus. In this chapter, we first provide a mini-review about decision-making processes during collective movements across primate species. We then summarize recent developments in the study of decision-making processes in Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana) at Huangshan, China, and place them in the context of the primate literature related to collective movement. These developments include the following key issues in collective movement in Tibetan macaques: leadership type, associated social factors, joining rules used, and the role of social relationships among group members in collective decision-making.
Download chapter PDF
1 Introduction
Group-living offers many benefits related to survival and reproduction for animals (Bertram 1978; van Schaik 1983; Zemel and Lubin 1995; Krause and Ruxton 2002). Nevertheless, it also involves some unavoidable costs, such as mate competition or interindividual conflict (Kappeler et al. 2015). Therefore, animals need to coordinate their actions and maintain group cohesion to gain the benefits of group-living (Conradt and Roper 2003; Fichtel et al. 2011). Group coordination is often difficult to achieve because individuals may differ in their needs and interests. When these differences cannot be reconciled, cohesiveness can be at risk (Rands et al. 2003, 2008). How consensus is achieved and implemented at the behavioral level is often studied during natural group movements to and from specific resources and/or locations (e.g., sleeping and foraging sites). Such studies can provide an ecologically relevant context to probe into fundamental mechanisms of social coordination of collective actions (Boinski and Garber 2000; Fichtel et al. 2011).
Social interactions could affect the process of group movements. For example, in large anonymous groups, such as fish schools and bird flocks, in which members do not know each other individually, direction and action during movements are regulated by self-coordination by individuals following the simple rule of keeping a certain distance to the nearest neighbor (Parrish and Edelstein-Keshet 1999; Couzin et al. 2002; Hemelrijk 2002). In contrast, in primate groups, where members know each other individually, certain dominant or affiliated individuals may play specific roles in the context of collective behavior, such as initiating or terminating a group movement (Boinski and Garber 2000; King and Cowlishaw 2009; King and Sueur 2011).
In primate species, initiation of a group movements can be accompanied by notifying behaviors (Kummer 1968) or preliminary behaviors (Sueur and Petit 2008a) that are exhibited in the pre-departure period, directly preceeding the group movement. This recruitment process often includes visual and acoustic communication, and which can influence the recruitment success of an initiation, i.e., whether the initiator is followed and, if so, by how many group members and how quickly (Sueur and Petit 2010; Seltmann et al. 2016; Sperber et al. 2017).
A successful collective movement can be driven by an unshared decision-making mechanism, i.e., one individual leading all group movements and other members following it all the time (Conradt and Roper 2005). In this decision-making process, the highest-ranking male usually plays a major role in leadership in several species of Old World monkey (Sueur and Petit 2008a). Alternatively, shared or partially shared decision-making mechanism can also result in a collective movement. That is, all group members or a subgroup can lead the movement of the entire group on different occasions (Pyritz et al. 2011). In this case, individuals with better social connections enjoy higher rates of initiating group movements (Sueur and Petit 2008b; Strandburg-Peshkin et al. 2015; Fratellone et al. 2018).
A major focus in the study of collective decisions in primates is the joining process, which occurs once an individual has initiated a collective movement. Often, primates do not decide independently on activity changes, but, rather, base their choices on the actions of their group mates. This form of joining rule, when one individual taking an action makes it more likely for another to do so as well, has been termed mimetism (Deneubourg and Goss 1989). Mimetism can be further categorized as anonymous mimetism and selective mimetism. Anonymous mimetism refers to individuals being more likely to take the actions of other individuals irrespective of their identity, and thus group movements can simply depend on the number of individuals who have already left or performed a certain behavior (Petit et al. 2009). Selective mimetism refers to joining decisions based on some other factors, such as distance, with individuals being more likely to join a movement when in close proximity (Ramseyer et al. 2009; Ward et al. 2013), and affiliation, with individuals being more likely to follow those group members with whom they have strong social bonds (King et al. 2011; Sueur et al. 2009, 2011; Seltmann et al. 2013; Strandburg-Peshkin et al. 2015; Farine et al. 2016).
Individuals may also engage another joining process based on a quorum rule. This rule states that once a minimum number of group members joins a movement, group movement will occur all the time (Conradt and Roper 2003; Wang et al. 2015; Rowe et al. 2018). A response to a quorum is observed when the probability of members exhibiting a vote by joining a movement depends on the number of individuals already performing the voting behavior (Pratt et al. 2002; Seeley and Visscher 2004; Sumpter 2006; Ward et al. 2008).
Interestingly in macaques, social style appears to influence the organization of group movements (Sueur and Petit 2008a). Social styles of the species of Macaca have been divided into four grades (Thierry 2000), ranging from grade 1 (the most intolerant) to grade 4 (the most tolerant). These styles appear to influence the initiation and joining process of group movements (Sueur and Petit 2008a). In species with a more despotic dominance style, decision-making is more likely to be unshared and social rank determines leadership (Sueur and Petit 2008a, b). In contrast, in species exhibiting a more egalitarian style, decision-making is more likely to be shared and social relationships determine leadership (Sueur and Petit 2008a). For instance, in rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese macaques (M. fuscata), both of which have a despotic social style, movements are mainly initiated by dominant individuals, and joining processes are also determined by dominance order (Sueur et al. 2009; Jacobs et al. 2011). In contrast, in macaques with a more egalitarian social style, such as Tonkean (M. tonkeana) and Barbary macaques (M. sylvanus), decision-making is equally or partially shared, and joining processes are determined by affiliation (Sueur et al. 2009; Jacobs et al. 2011; Seltmann et al. 2013).
Currently, the relationship between social style and leadership and other important aspects of collective decision-making has been investigated only in a limited number of macaque species. Therefore, detailed studies of collective movement in relatively less known species, such as the Tibetan macaque (M. thibetana), are of special interest for a better understanding of the link between social relationships and collective decision-making. We therefore take this opportunity to review and synthesize information based largely on our studies of Tibetan macaques. We hope that some of the findings and insights (including those that have not been fully developed in our publications) will enrich our understanding of decision-making processes in collective movement in primates in general and macaques in particular.
2 Collective Decision-Making in Tibetan Macaques
2.1 A Macaque Species for Studying Decision-Making
Because Macaca species vary in dominance style (Thierry et al. 2004), they serve as an interesting taxon to study how social relationship influences the process of group movements (Jacobs et al. 2011). Tibetan macaques are highly gregarious and live in cohesive groups (Li 1999). Similar to other macaques, Tibetan macaques show female philopatry, male dispersal, and linear dominance hierarchies (Berman et al. 2004).
The wild group of macaques we studied (YA1, see Chap. 2 for detailed information about the history, demography, and habitat of the study group) engaged in social activities in nearby forest during most of the day without any restriction on their home range. For the convenience of viewing by tourists, they were supplied with 3–4 kg of corn daily (Berman and Li 2002; Berman et al. 2008; Xia et al. 2012). After corn feeding, they regularly switched locations from the feeding site to the nearby forest. Collective movements often occurred at the time of the switch. We therefore investigated decision-making processes during group movements in this group from August to December of 2012.
2.2 Decision-Making During the Initiation Process of Group Movements
It is still debated whether Tibetan macaques exhibit a despotic or a tolerant dominance style (Thierry 2000; Berman et al. 2004). In accordance with a despotic dominance style (Berman et al. 2004), Tibetan macaques should be expected to show an unshared decision-making process when initiating group movements, i.e., a single, highest-ranking individual leads most movements. In reality, however, Tibetan macaques demonstrate a shared decision-making process with affiliative individuals more often initiating group movements than less sociable group members (Wang et al. 2016). Considering the above contradiction about dominance style in Tibetan macaques, we assume that there may be a potential connection between social rank/affiliative relationship and the initiation of group movement.
We observed initiation processes when Tibetan macaques returned from the feeding site to the nearby forest. Thus, an initiator was defined as the first individual that moved more than 10 m in less than 30 s from the provisioning area to the forest. Any individual walking more than 5 m and within 45° of the direction taken by the initiator was considered as a follower (Sueur and Petit 2008a). We used the criterion of 5 min for each successive follower who joined the movement after the first mover or previous follower (Wang et al. 2016). In our study, only those movements including at least two-thirds of group members were counted as successful group movements. To quantify leadership in group movements, we standardized initiation data on the number of initiations of each individual by the number of times in which this individual was identified at the provisioning area (Wang et al. 2016).
During the 5 months of our observation period, we recorded more than 200 initiation attempts, all of them by adult members. Two-thirds of these initiations were considered as successful group movements. We found that all adults could initiate group movements, but that they differed significantly in the standardized number of successful initiations. This result clearly showed that decision-making during the initiation process of group movement was shared among adults.
To explore which factors might affect collective decision-making, we analyzed the relationship between several key biological/social attributes and the initiation of group movement. Interestingly, there was no significant difference in the standardized number of successful initiations between adult males and females. Second, there was neither a correlation between social rank and the standardized number of successful initiations nor with the success ratio of initiations. Also, age of adults was not correlated with the successful initiation of group movement (Fig. 5.1).
To evaluate the relationship between social affiliation of an adult and its leadership in group movements, we related the number and ratio of successful initiations of every subject in the provisioning area to its eigenvector centrality coefficient based on proximity relations among group members when they were in the forest (in comparison with the situations when they were in the feeding site). We used focal animal sampling method to collect proximity data for assessing affiliative relationships among group members (Altmann 1974; Li 1999; Berman et al. 2008). We found a positive correlation between the eigenvector centrality coefficient (based on proximity relations) and the standardized number of successful initiations across adults. Eigenvector centrality was also correlated positively with the success ratio of initiations.
Moreover, to further analyze the relationship between affiliated behavior and leadership of group movement, we correlated the initiation of group movement with social grooming among adults. Results showed that the standardized number of successful initiations was positively correlated with the duration of social grooming, including grooming given and grooming received (Fig. 5.2).
Correlation between successful initiations of group movements and social grooming in Tibetan macaques. Proportion of time spent in social grooming indicates the duration of grooming of every subject divided by its total focal observation time. The two lines respectively represent positive correlations between grooming given/grooming received and successful initiations of group movements
2.3 Decision-Making During the Joining Process of Group Movements
Joining processes were also observed when Tibetan macaques returned from the feeding site, where they were regularly provisioned, to the nearby forest. We tested whether joining occurs according to a quorum decision or mimetism as shown in a simple schematic to illustrate how the two responses would differ (Fig. 5.3).
A schematic depiction of mimetism and quorum. Nos. 1, 2, and 3 in red circles represent individuals who have joined the movement. Nos. 4, 5, and 6 are individuals waiting to join the movement. Dotted line of upper figure indicates selective mimetism: Nos. 4, 5, and 6 would join the movement based on the choice of specific members (e.g., No. 1 is the highest-ranking male). Solid line of upper figure indicates anonymous mimetism: Nos. 4, 5, and 6 would join the movement based on the number of joiners by linearly. Threshold of lower figure indicates quorum: for example, once half of the members (i.e., three individuals) have joined the movement, other individuals would follow the collective action all the time
During our preliminary observation of group YA1 (August 1–14, 2012), 5 min were used as the minimum duration of initiating a successful group movement. Therefore, an early joiner was defined as an individual that moved in the first 5 min after the initiator departed (Wang et al. 2015). According to this definition, an initiator in our study was also considered as an early joiner because he/she left in the first 5 min.
We assessed the relationship between the number of adult early joiners and the probability of successful group movement. The results revealed that group movements were not successful unless three or more early joiners participated in the movement. When three to six early joiners participated, successful group movements occurred without a consistent pattern, showing some fluctuations in the probability of successful group movement. Nonetheless, once more than half of the early joiners participated in the movement, other group members followed the collective actions all the time.
To further study the role of early joiners in group movements, we performed a correlation analysis between the mean joining order and eigenvector centrality coefficient for adults in group movements (Wang et al. 2015). Results showed that the earlier an individual joined the movement, the higher its centrality was (Fig. 5.4). We then explored key attributes of early joiners in the social network of those in group movements. Our results showed that early joiners differed significantly in eigenvector centrality coefficient based on the half-weight index (HWI: co-occurrence index in group movements, Wang et al. 2015), but there was no difference between adult males and females. Also, age and eigenvector centrality coefficients were not correlated. However, social rank was positively correlated with eigenvector centrality coefficients in both adult males and females.
Finally, we compared two eigenvector centrality coefficients of individuals in group movements and in proximity relations (Wang et al. 2015). We found a positive correlation between the two coefficients (Fig. 5.5). This result indicates a close relation between affiliative behavior and the joining process of group movements.
3 Social Relationship and Collective Decision-Making
Our collection of studies revealed that all adult Tibetan macaques had opportunities to successfully initiate group movements. This result is consistent with studies in Barbary macaques (Seltmann et al. 2013) and Tonkean macaques (Sueur and Petit 2008a), both of which have a tolerant social style. However, different adults had varying times of success in initiating group movements across the study period. Eigenvector centrality coefficients were positively correlated with the number of successful initiation of group movements.
Our data demonstrate that more affiliative individuals were more likely to assume leadership roles. The importance of social relationship is consistent with the results of several other studies (e.g., Tonkean macaques: Sueur and Petit 2008a, b; Sueur et al. 2009). King et al. (2011) suggested that chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) with higher eigenvector centrality coefficients in their social network were more likely to attract partners to follow their initiations. We assume that individuals preferentially followed “friends” because following friends may be associated with benefits. For instance, female baboons (P. cynocephalus) that had closer bonds with others lived longer and their offspring also had a higher probability to survive (Silk et al. 2010), but in Tibetan macaques neither these friendships nor their potential consequences have been studied yet.
We found a positive correlation between the number of successful initiations and the amount of time spent grooming given/received, suggesting that the motivation to move may lay in staying away from the provisioning area and moving to the relatively calm forest, where animals could engage in social grooming. We carried out our study at a tourist site (Wang et al. 2016), and adult Tibetan macaques at our study site had indeed higher rates of aggression in the provisioning area than in the forest (Berman et al. 2007). Moreover, as no large predators were present at the study site in recent decades (Li 1999), the forest was supposed to be a safer and less stressful location for monkeys to engage in affiliative behaviors such as grooming. We see the location change from the provisioning area to forest as an adaptive response to the socioecological conditions experienced by our study group.
Our work on the joining process of group movements has provided several important insights into which rules might be used by Tibetan macaques in decision-making during collective movement. First, we found that the more central a group member was in the social network, the earlier it participated in a movement, as shown by the positive correlation between the mean joining order of every joiner and its eigenvector centrality coefficient (Wang et al. 2015). This result is comparable with situations in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra), where females at the front of a group movement have the highest centrality eigenvectors among the adult group members (Belle et al. 2013). Our results demonstrate the importance of early joiners in the decision-making process and indicate that the initiator was not always the only decision-maker. Other joiners in the first 5 min might also play a key role in decision-making.
Second, we found that higher-ranking early joiners tended to have higher eigenvector centrality coefficients. Because the eigenvector centrality coefficient can quantify the attraction of early joiners to other members during the joining process (Newman 2004), our data (Wang et al. 2015) showed the role of social rank in early joiners. That is, higher-ranking joiners could have more companions in group movements. A similar influence of social rank on decision-making has also been reported in other species. For instance, alpha males have been reported to be the consistent decision-makers in group movements in mountain gorillas, Gorilla gorilla (Watts 2000).
Third, we found that early joiners who had higher centrality coefficients in proximity activities also had higher centrality coefficients during collective movements. This means that early joiners with frequent social interactions could also attract more members during the joining process. This result is comparable to findings in chacma baboons and red-fronted lemurs, Eulemur rufifrons (Stueckle and Zinner 2008; King et al. 2011; Sperber et al. 2019).
Our data also showed that when the number of early joiners in a group was below seven, group members preferred to follow higher-ranking or affiliative early joiners during the joining process of group movements. By this time, selective mimetism had most likely been used as the joining rule. Apart from our study, selective mimetism has also been suggested in several other studies (Detrain et al. 1999; Camazine et al. 2001; Couzin and Krause 2003; Sumpter 2006; Gautrais et al. 2007; Sperber et al. 2019). In Tonkean macaques, for instance, how an individual decides to join a collective movement depends on whether it is strongly affiliated to departing individuals (Sueur et al. 2009).
We can explain selective mimetism in Tibetan macaques with respect to their social style. For example, Tonkean macaques exhibit an egalitarian social structure (Sueur and Petit 2008b). In this species, individuals decide when to move via a quorum. The lack of centrality for dominant or old Tonkean macaques suggests that all individuals may have equal weight in the voting process and interactions are not constrained by individual status (Sueur and Petit 2008b). However, rhesus macaques, with a despotic style, prefer to join high-ranking or related individuals during collective movements, showing selective mimetism (Sueur and Petit 2008a, b; Sueur et al. 2010). In Tibetan macaques, higher-ranking early joiners were socially connected to more individuals than lower-ranking group members, both in collective movements and during proximity activities. As our results showed that high-ranking individuals had high centrality coefficients based on the co-occurrence index in group movements, they were more attractive to other members than low-ranking individuals during the joining process.
Our data also showed that when the number of early joiners had accumulated to more than half of the adults in our study group, all group members would participate in group movement all the time. Clearly, this threshold of “>50% adult members” indicates the existence of another joining rule, the quorum rule, during collective movements in Tibetan macaques. The voting process in the group of Tibetan macaques we studied may be explained by the reduction in the risk of being left behind from the group (Wang et al. 2015). A similar joining rule has also been found in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus), rhesus macaques, and Tonkean macaques, all of which use the threshold of four in collective movements (Petit et al. 2009; Sueur and Petit 2010).
4 Conclusions
In this chapter, we reviewed and synthesized studies of collective movement and decision-making in Tibetan macaques. We suggest that leadership of group movements in Tibetan macaques was distributed among adults rather than exclusively taken by a single, high-ranking individual. Different members led the group on different occasions, and social relationships were more related to leadership than social rank, age, or sex. Performing group movement presumably produced opportunities to switch locations for adults to participate in other social activities, including social grooming. Moreover, social relationship also mattered for the joining process. Tibetan macaques used selective mimetism and a quorum process in collective decision-making, and early joiners with closer affiliation played a critical role as to which rule was used. Thus, our study provided further evidence for the link between social relationships and collective decision-making in a little known macaque species. Future studies can examine whether social relationships affect the decision-making process in Tibetan macaques at the same level when group size varies. This will lead us to a better understanding as to whether a general pattern exists for group coordination and social cohesion through collective decision-making.
References
Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267
Belle SV, Estrada A, Garber PA (2013) Collective group movement and leadership in wild black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67:31–41
Berman CM, Li JH (2002) Impact of translocation, provisioning and range restriction on a group of Macaca thibetana. Int J Primatol 23:383–397
Berman CM, Ionica C, Li JH (2004) Dominance style among Macaca thibetana on Mt. Huangshan, China. Int J Primatol 25:1283–1312
Berman CM, Li JH, Ogawa H, Ionica C, Yin HB (2007) Primate tourism, range restriction, and infant risk among Macaca thibetana at Mt. Huangshan, China. Int J Primatol 28:1123–1141
Berman CM, Ogawa H, Ionica C, Yin HB, Li JH (2008) Variation in kin bias over time in a group of Tibetan macaques at Huangshan, China: contest competition, time constraints or risk response? Behaviour 145:863–896
Bertram BCR (1978) Living in groups: predators and prey. In: Krebs JR, Davies JB (eds) Behavioural ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 64–96
Boinski S, Garber PA (2000) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL
Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E (2001) Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
Conradt L, Roper TJ (2003) Group decision-making in animals. Nature 421:155–158
Conradt L, Roper TJ (2005) Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol Evol 20:449–456
Couzin ID, Krause J (2003) Self-organization and collective behavior in vertebrates. Adv Stud Behav 32:1–75
Couzin ID, Krause J, James R, Ruxton GD, Franks NR (2002) Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. J Theor Biol 218:1–11
Deneubourg JL, Goss S (1989) Collective patterns and decision-making. Ital J Zool 1:295–311
Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM (1999) Decision-making in foraging by social insects. In: Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM (eds) Information processing in social insects. Birkhaüser Verlag, Basel, pp 331–354
Farine DR, Strandburg-Peshkin A, Berger-Wolf T, Ziebart B, Brugere I, Li J, Crofoot MC (2016) Both nearest neighbours and long-term affiliates predict individual locations during collective movement in wild baboons. Sci Rep 6:27704
Fichtel C, Pyritz L, Kappeler PM (2011) Coordination of group movements in non-human primates. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Kappeler P, Ellwart T (eds) Coordination in human and primate groups. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 37–56
Fratellone GP, Li JH, Sheeran LK, Wagner RS, Wang X, Sun L (2018) Social connectivity among female Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana) increases the speed of collective movements. Primates 60(3):183–189. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-018-0691-6
Gautrais J, Michelena P, Sibbald A, Bon R, Deneubourg JL (2007) Allelomimetic synchronization in merino sheep. Anim Behav 74:1443–1454
Hemelrijk CK (2002) Understanding social behaviour with the help of complexity science. Ethology 108:655–671
Jacobs A, Watanabe K, Petit O (2011) Social structure affects initiations of group movements but not recruitment success in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Int J Primatol 32:1311–1324
Kappeler PM, Cremer S, Nunn CL (2015) Sociality and health: impacts of sociality on disease susceptibility and transmission in animal and human societies. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 370:20140116
King AJ, Cowlishaw G (2009) Leaders, followers and group decision-making. Commun Integr Biol 2:147–150
King AJ, Sueur C (2011) Where next? Group coordination and collective decision making by primates. Int J Primatol 32:1245–1267
King AJ, Sueur C, Huchard E, Cowlishaw G (2011) A rule-of-thumb based on social affiliation explains collective movements in desert baboons. Anim Behav 82:1337–1345
Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Kummer H (1968) Social organisation of hamadryas baboons. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL
Li JH (1999) The Tibetan macaque society: a field study. Anhui University Press, Hefei. (In Chinese)
Newman MEJ (2004) Analysis of weighted networks. Phys Rev E 70:056131
Parrish JK, Edelstein-Keshet L (1999) Complexity, pattern, and evolutionary trade-offs in animal aggregation. Science 284:99–101
Petit O, Gautrais J, Leca JB, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg JL (2009) Collective decision-making in white-faced capuchin monkeys. Proc R Soc B 276:3495–3503
Pratt SC, Mallon EB, Sumpter DJT, Franks NR (2002) Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52:117–127
Pyritz LW, Kappeler PM, Fichtel C (2011) Coordination of group movements in wild red-fronted lemurs: processes and influence of ecological and reproductive seasonality. Int J Primatol 32:1325–1347
Ramseyer A, Boissy A, Dumont B, Thierry B (2009) Decision making in group departures of sheep is a continuous process. Anim Behav 78:71–78
Rands SA, Cowlishaw G, Pettifor RA, Rowcliffe JM, Johnstone RA (2003) Spontaneous emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs. Nature 423:432–434
Rands SA, Cowlishaw G, Pettifor RA, Rowcliffe JM, Johnstone RA (2008) The emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs when the qualities of individuals differ. BMC Evol Biol 8:51
Rowe AK, Li JH, Sun L, Sheeran LK, Wagner RS, Xia DP, Uhey DA, Chen R (2018) Collective decision making in Tibetan macaques: how followers affect the rules and speed of group movement. Anim Behav 146:51–61
Seeley TD, Visscher PK (2004) Quorum sensing during nest-site selection by honeybee swarms. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56:594–601
Seltmann A, Majolo B, Schülke O, Ostner J (2013) The organization of collective group movements in wild Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): dominance style drives processes of group coordination in macaques. PLoS One 8:e67285
Seltmann A, Franz M, Majolo B, Qarro M, Ostner J, Schülke O (2016) Recruitment and monitoring behaviors by leaders predict following in wild Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Primate Biol 3:23–31
Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol 20:1359–1361
Sperber AL, Werner LM, Kappeler PM, Fichtel C (2017) Grunt to go – vocal coordination of group movements in redfronted lemurs. Ethology 123:894–905
Sperber AL, Kappeler PM, Fichtel C (2019) Should I stay or should I go? Individual movement decisions during group departures in redfronted lemurs. Proc R Soc Open Sci 6(3). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.180991
Strandburg-Peshkin A, Farine DR, Couzin ID, Crofoot MC (2015) Group decisions. Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons. Science 348:1358–1361
Stueckle S, Zinner D (2008) To follow or not to follow: decision making and leadership during the morning departure in chacma baboons. Anim Behav 75:1995–2004
Sueur C, Petit O (2008a) Shared or unshared consensus decision in macaques? Behav Process 78:84–92
Sueur C, Petit O (2008b) Organization of group members at departure is driven by dominance style in Macaca. Int J Primatol 29:1085–1098
Sueur C, Petit O (2010) Signals use by leaders in Macaca tonkeana and Macaca mulatta: group-mate recruitment and behaviour monitoring. Anim Cogn 13:239–248
Sueur C, Petit O, Deneubourg JL (2009) Selective mimetism at departure in collective movements of Macaca tonkeana: an experimental and theoretical approach. Anim Behav 78:1087–1095
Sueur C, Deneubourg JL, Petit O (2010) Sequence of quorums during collective decision making in macaques. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1875–1885
Sueur C, Deneubourg J-L, Petit O (2011) From the first intention movement to the last joiner: macaques combine mimetic rules to optimize their collective decisions. Proc R Soc B 278:1697–1704
Sumpter DJT (2006) The principles of collective animal behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 361:5–22
Thierry B (2000) Covariation of conflict management patterns across macaque species. In: Aureli F, de Waal FBM (eds) Natural conflict resolution. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, pp 106–128
Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (2004) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
van Schaik CP (1983) Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87:120–144
Wang X, Sun L, Li JH, Xia DP, Sun BH, Zhang D (2015) Collective movement in the Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana): early joiners write the rule of the game. PLoS One 10:e0127459
Wang X, Sun L, Sheeran LK, Sun BH, Zhang QX, Zhang D, Xia DP, Li JH (2016) Social rank versus affiliation: which is more closely related to leadership of group movements in Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana)? Am J Primatol 78:816–824
Ward AJW, Sumpter DJT, Couzin ID, Hart PJB, Krause J (2008) Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:6948–6953
Ward AJW, Herbert-Read JE, Jordan LA, James R, Krause J, Ma Q, Rubenstein DI, Sumpter DJT, Morrell LJ (2013) Initiators, leaders, and recruitment mechanisms in the collective movements of damselfish. Am Nat 181:748–760
Watts D (2000) Mountain gorilla habitat use strategies and group movements. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp 351–374
Xia DP, Li JH, Garber PA, Sun LX, Zhu Y, Sun BH (2012) Grooming reciprocity in female Tibetan macaques Macaca thibetana. Am J Primatol 74:569–579
Zemel A, Lubin Y (1995) Inter-group competition and stable group sizes. Anim Behav 50:485–488
Acknowledgments
We thank the Huangshan Monkey Management Center and the Huangshan Garden Forest Bureau for their permission for us to conduct research at the field site. We are also grateful to Haibin Cheng’s family for their outstanding logistic support to our study. This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31801983; 31772475; 31672307; 31401981; 31372215), the Initial Foundation of Doctoral Scientific Research in Anhui University (J01003268), the Training Program for Excellent Young Teachers (J05011709) and China Scholarship Council.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter's Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the chapter's Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.
Copyright information
© 2020 The Author(s)
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Wang, X., Fichtel, C., Sun, L., Li, JH. (2020). Social Relationships Impact Collective Decision-Making in Tibetan Macaques. In: Li, JH., Sun, L., Kappeler, P. (eds) The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque. Fascinating Life Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27920-2_5
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27920-2_5
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-030-27919-6
Online ISBN: 978-3-030-27920-2
eBook Packages: Biomedical and Life SciencesBiomedical and Life Sciences (R0)