Skip to main content
Log in

A scoping review and index of body stimuli in psychological science

  • Original Manuscript
  • Published:
Behavior Research Methods Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Naturalistic body stimuli are necessary for understanding many aspects of human psychology, yet there are no centralized databases of body stimuli. Furthermore, there are a high number of independently developed stimulus sets lacking in standardization and reproducibility potential, and a general lack of organization, contributing to issues of both replicability and generalizability in body-related research. We conducted a comprehensive scoping review to index and explore existing naturalistic whole-body stimuli. Our research questions were as follows: (1) What sets of naturalistic human whole-body stimuli are present in the literature? And (2) On what factors (e.g., demographics, emotion expression) do these stimuli vary? To be included, stimulus sets had to (1) include human bodies as stimuli; (2) be photographs, videos, or other depictions of real human bodies (not computer generated, drawn, etc.); (3) include the whole body (defined as torso, arms, and legs); and (4) could include edited images, but still had to be recognizable as human bodies. We identified a relatively large number of existing stimulus sets (N = 79) which offered relative variability in terms of main manipulated factors and the degree of visual information included (i.e., inclusion of heads and/or faces). However, stimulus sets were demographically homogenous, skewed towards White, young adult, and female bodies. We identified significant issues in reporting and availability practices, posing a challenge to the generalizability, reliability, and reproducibility of body-related research. Accordingly, we urge researchers to adopt transparent and accessible practices and to take steps to diversify body stimuli.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Code availability

Not applicable.

Data Availability

Data are publicly available at https://osf.io/mz54e/?view_only=754ad2a7646c4dbebc3b709e9044b3f4.

Notes

  1. OSF does not have a built-in preregistration form for scoping reviews. We used van den Akker et al.’s (2020) Inclusive Systematic Review Registration Form (v0.92) and uploaded a stable PDF before commencing the search.

  2. Headless bodies were included in the review by these inclusion criteria. The choice to include headless bodies was driven both by similarities to analogous situations with naturalistic facial stimuli – which often exclude some facial information or are portrayed as bodiless heads/faces – and by evidence of similar visual processing of bodies with and without heads (e.g., in a recent meta-analysis of the body inversion effected which yielded no summary differences in the inversion effect for headless bodies versus bodies with heads (e.g., Alexi et al., 2019; Griffin & Oswald, 2022).

  3. When coding stimulus clothing, our coding was guided by Griffin and Oswald’s (2022) skin-to-clothing ratio, though we degree of clothing (rather than degree of skin visible) for clarity. Accordingly, minimal clothing referred to bodies which were presented in underwear or swimwear, moderate clothing referred to bodies presented in shorts and tanktops or similar-coverage clothing, and fully clothed referred to bodies presented in trousers and shirts or clothing with more coverage (see Griffin & Oswald, 2022).

  4. We present our results rounded to the nearest whole number for clarity.

  5. A number of papers reported a range of ages rather than a mean age. In those cases, we report the range in Table 1, but for the purposes of this calculation took the median of the range as our estimate.

References

*indicates references included in the review

  • Abo Foul, Y., Eitan, R., & Aviezer, H. (2018). Perceiving emotionally incongruent cues from faces and bodies: Older adults get the whole picture. Psychology and Aging, 33(4), 660–666. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000255

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akin, D., & Huang, L. M. (2019). Perceptions of college students with disabilities. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 32(1), 21–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Aleong, R., Duchesne, S., & Paus, T. (2007). Assessment of adolescent body perception: Development and characterization of a novel tool for morphing images of adolescent bodies. Behavior Research Methods39(3), 651–666.https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193037

  • *Alexi, J., Cleary, D., Dommisse, K., Palermo, R., Kloth, N., Burr, D., & Bell, J. (2018). Past visual experiences weigh in on body size estimation. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 215.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-18418-3

  • Alexi, J., Dommisse, K., Cleary, D., Palermo, R., Kloth, N., & Bell, J. (2019). An assessment of computer-generated stimuli for use in studies of body size estimation and bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 2390. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02390

  • Alt, N. P., Lick, D. J., Hunger, J. M., & Johnson, K. L. (2019). Evaluative implications of intersecting body weight and other social categories: The role of typicality. Body Image, 31, 19–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.08.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., & Weisbuch, M. (2010). Nonverbal behavior. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 464–497). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470561119.socpsy001013

  • American Psychological Association. (2022). Bias-free language: Gender. https://apastyle.apa.org/style-grammar-guidelines/bias-free-language/gender

  • Arksey, H., & O’Malley, L. (2005). Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 8(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnett. (2008). The neglected 95: Why American psychology needs to become less American. The American Psychologist63(7), 602–614.https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602

  • *Ashbridge, E., Perrett, D. I., Oram, M. W., & Jellema, T. (2000). Effect of image orientation and size on object recognition: Responses of single units in the macaque monkey temporal cortex. Cognitive Neuropsychology17(1-3), 13-34.https://doi.org/10.1080/026432900380463

  • *Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33(6), 717–746.https://doi.org/10.1068/p5096

  • *Bauer, A., Schneider, S., Waldorf, M., Cordes, M., Huber, T. J., Braks, K., & Vocks, S. (2017). Visual processing of one’s own body over the course of time: Evidence for the vigilance-avoidance theory in adolescents with anorexia nervosa? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 50(10), 1205–1213.https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22771

  • Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Bushman, B. J. (2023). A review of multisite replication projects in social psychology: Is it viable to sustain any confidence in social psychology’s knowledge base? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 18(4), 912–935. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221121815

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Baur, J., Krohmer, K., Naumann, E., & Svaldi, J. (2022). Attentional processing of body images in women with overweight and obesity. Advance online publication [preprint]. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-1486031/v1

  • *Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (2002). Parallel visual motion processing streams for manipulable objects and human movements. Neuron34(1), 149–159.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00642-6

  • *Biotti, F., Ahmad, S., Quinn, R., & Brewer, R. (2022). Development and validation of the Interoceptive States Static Images (ISSI) database. Behavior Research Methods, 54, 1744–1756.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01706-2

  • *Blechert, J., Ansorge, U., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2010). A body-related dot-probe task reveals distinct attentional patterns for bulimia nervosa and anorexia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 575–585.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019531

  • *Blechert, J., Nickert, T., Caffier, D., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2009). Social comparison and its relation to body dissatisfaction in bulimia nervosa: Evidence from eye movements. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71(8), 907–912.https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181b4434d

  • *Brooks, K. R., Mond, J. M., Stevenson, R. J., & Stephen, I. D. (2016). Body image distortion and exposure to extreme body types: Contingent adaptation and cross adaptation for self and other. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00334

  • *Butti, N., Finisguerra, A., & Urgesi, C. (2022). Holistic processing of body stimuli: Evidence of body composite illusion in adults and children. Developmental Psychology, 58(7), 1286–1297.https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001353

  • *Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi007

  • Chen, J. M., Norman, J. B., & Nam, Y. (2021). Broadening the stimulus set: Introducing the american multiracial faces database. Behavior Research Methods, 53, 371–389. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01447-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheon, B. K., Melani, I., & Hong, Y.-Y. (2020). How USA-centric is psychology? An archival study of implicit assumptions of generalizability of findings to human nature based on origins of study samples. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(7), 928–937. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620927269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Cho, A., Kwak, S.-M., & Lee, J.-H. (2013). Identifying attentional bias and emotional response after appearance-related stimuli exposure. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(1), 50–55.https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0223

  • *Christensen, J. F., Nadal, M., Cela-Conde, C. J., & Gomila, A. (2014). A norming study and library of 203 dance movements. Perception, 43(2–3), 178–206.https://doi.org/10.1068/p7581

  • *Connor, P., Varney, J., Keltner, D., & Chen, S. (2021). Social class competence stereotypes are amplified by socially signaled economic inequality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin47(1), 89–105.https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220916640

  • *Cordes, M., Vocks, S., Düsing, R., & Waldorf, M. (2017). Effects of the exposure to self- and other-referential bodies on state body image and negative affect in resistance-trained men. Body Image, 21, 57–65.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.02.007

  • *Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. NeuroImage, 31(3), 1257–1267.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033

  • *Cross, E. S., Mackie, E. C., Wolford, G., & de C. Hamilton, A. F. (2010). Contorted and ordinary body postures in the human brain. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 397–407.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2093-x

  • Dawel, A., Miller, E. J., Horsburgh, A., & Ford, P. (2021). A systematic survey of face stimuli used in psychological research 2000–2020. Behavior Research Methods, 54(4), 1889–1901. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01705-3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Smith, F. G., & Little, A. C. (2010). Are attractive men’s faces masculine or feminine? The importance of controlling confounds in face stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(3), 751–758. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016457

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine, L., Holzleitner, I., Tiddeman, B., & Jones, B. C. (2022). Reproducible methods for face research. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/j2754

  • de Castro, M. R., Morgado, F. F. R., & Freitas, I. F. (2018). Brazilian photographic figure rating scale for men: Psychometric investigation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 125(3), 493–506. https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512518757351

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N. (2004). Fear fosters flight: A mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(47), 16701–16706.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0407042101

  • de Gelder, B. (2009). Why bodies? Twelve reasons for including bodily expressions in affective neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 364(1535), 3475–3484. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *de Gelder, B., & Van den Stock, J. (2011). The bodily expressive action stimulus test (BEAST). Construction and validation of a stimulus basis for measuring perception of whole body expression of emotions. Frontiers in Psychology2, 181. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00181

  • de Gelder, B., Van den Stock, J., Meeren, H. K. M., Sinke, C. B. A., Kret, M. E., & Tamietto, M. (2010). Standing up for the body. Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in the perception of bodies and bodily expressions. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 513–527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.008

  • de Gelder, B. (2013). From body perception to action preparation: A distributed neural system for viewing bodily expressions of emotion. In K. Johnson & M. Shiffrar (Eds.), People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception (pp. 350–368). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Del Zotto, M., Framorando, D., & Pegna, A. J. (2020). Waist‐to‐hip ratio affects female body attractiveness and modulates early brain responses. European Journal of Neuroscience, 52(11), 4490–4498.https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14209

  • *Devlin, A. L., & Wilson, P. H. (2010). Adult age differences in the ability to mentally transform object and body stimuli. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 17(6), 709–729.https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2010.510554

  • *Diekhoff, D. (2017). A mixed-method, factorial examination of men's and women's perceptual and affective responses to ideal, actual, and average male and female bodies (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas Woman’s University). https://twu-ir.tdl.org/handle/11274/9776

  • D'ignazio, C., & Klein, L. F. (2020). Data feminism. MIT Press.

  • *Downing, P. E., Chan, A. W.-Y., Peelen, M. V., Dodds, C. M., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Domain specificity in visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(10), 1453–1461.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj086

  • *Downing, P. E., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293(5539), 2470–2473.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1063414

  • Fox, J., Pearce, K. E., Massanari, A. L., Riles, J. M., Szulc, Ł, Ranjit, Y. S., Trevisan, S., Soriano, C. R. R., Vitak, J., Arora, P., Ahn, S. J. G., Alper, M., Gambino, A., Gonzalez, C., Lynch, T., Williamson, L. D., & Gonzales, A. L. (2021). Open science, closed doors? Countering marginalization through an agenda for ethical, inclusive research in communication. Journal of Communication, 71(5), 764–784. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqab029

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Friederich, H. C., Kumari, V., Uher, R., Riga, M., Schmidt, U., Campbell, I. C., Herzog, W., & Treasure, J. (2006). Differential motivational responses to food and pleasurable cues in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: A startle reflex paradigm. Psychological Medicine, 36(9), 1327–1335.https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706008129

  • *Gao, J., Kawakami, F., & Tomonaga, M. (2020). Body perception in chimpanzees and humans: The expert effect. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 7148.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63876-x

  • *Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions. NeuroImage, 35(2), 959–967.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.030

  • Griffin, J. W., & Oswald, F. (2022). A multilevel Bayesian meta-analysis of the body inversion effect: Evaluating controversies over headless and sexualized bodies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. Advance online publication.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02067-3

  • *Groves, K., Kennett, S., & Gillmeister, H. (2017). Evidence for ERP biomarkers of eating disorder symptoms in women. Biological Psychology, 123, 205–219.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.12.016

  • *Groves, K., Kennett, S., & Gillmeister, H. (2018). Affective responses to body stimuli: Comparing male and female bodies with cropped heads and masked faces. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 30(7), 754–770.https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2018.1518962

  • *Groves, K., Kennett, S., & Gillmeister, H. (2020). Evidence for altered configural body processing in women at risk of disorders characterized by body image disturbance. British Journal of Psychology, 111(3), 508–535.https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12412

  • Guyan, K. (2022). Queer data: Using gender, sex, and sexuality data for action. Bloomsbury.

  • *Habacha, H., Moreau, D., Jarraya, M., Lejeune-Poutrain, L., & Molinaro, C. (2018). Dissociating object-based from egocentric transformations in mental body rotation: Effect of stimuli size. Experimental Brain Research, 236(1), 275–284.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5125-y

  • Handron, C., Kirby, T. A., Wang, J., Matskewich, H. E., & Cheryan, S. (2017). Unexpected gains: Being overweight buffers Asian Americans from prejudice against foreigners. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1214–1227. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617720912

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Harris, C. V., Bradlyn, A. S., Coffman, J., Gunel, E., & Cottrell, L. (2008). BMI-based body size guides for women and men: Development and validation of a novel pictorial method to assess weight-related concepts. International Journal of Obesity, 32(2), 336–342.https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803704

  • *Harry, B. B., Umla-Runge, K., Lawrence, A. D., Graham, K. S., & Downing, P. E. (2016). Evidence for integrated visual face and body representations in the anterior temporal lobes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(8), 1178–1193.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00966

  • *Hartmann, M., Falconer, C. J., & Mast, F. W. (2011). Imagined paralysis impairs embodied spatial transformations. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 155–162.https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2011.594498

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 29–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Heron, M., & Slaughter, V. (2010). Infants’ responses to real humans and representations of humans. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34(1), 34–45.https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025409345047

  • Hester, N., & Hehman, E. (2023). Dress is a fundamental component of person perception. Personality and Social Psychology Review. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683231157961

  • *Hodzic, A., Kaas, A., Muckli, L., Stirn, A., & Singer, W. (2009). Distinct cortical networks for the detection and identification of human body. NeuroImage, 45(4), 1264–1271.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.027

  • *Horndasch, S., Heinrich, H., Kratz, O., Mai, S., Graap, H., & Moll, G. H. (2015). Perception and evaluation of women’s bodies in adolescents and adults with anorexia nervosa. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265(8), 677–687.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-015-0603-3

  • Hu, Y., Parde, C. J., Hill, M. Q., Mahmood, N., & O’Toole, A. J. (2018). First impressions of personality traits from body shapes. Psychological Science, 29(12), 1969–1983. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618799300

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iseki, K., Hanakawa, T., Shinozaki, J., Nankaku, M., & Fukuyama, H. (2008). Neural mechanisms involved in mental imagery and observation of gait. NeuroImage, 41(3), 1021–1031. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, B. T. (2021). Toward a more transparent, rigorous, and generative psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 147(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000317

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. L., Gill, S., Reichman, V., & Tassinary, L. G. (2007). Swagger, sway, and sexuality: Judging sexual orientation from body motion and morphology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(3), 321–334. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.321

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. L., & Iida, M. (2013). Person (mis) perception? On the biased representation of the human body. In K. Johnson & M. Shiffrar (Eds.), People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception (pp. 203–219). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K., & Shiffrar, M. (Eds.). (2013). People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception. Oxford University Press.

  • Johnson, K. L., & Shiffrar, M. (2013). Making great strides: Advances in research on the perception of the human body. In K. Johnson & M. Shiffrar (Eds.), People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception (pp. 3–10). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Kaltner, S., Riecke, B. E., & Jansen, P. (2014). Embodied mental rotation: A special link between egocentric transformation and the bodily self. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00505

  • Kastner, M., Tricco, A. C., Soobiah, C., Lillie, E., Perrier, L., Horsley, T., Welch, V., Cogo, E., Antony, J., & Straus, S. E. (2012). What is the most appropriate knowledge synthesis method to conduct a review? Protocol for a scoping review. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 12(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-12-114

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Khalil, H., Peters, M. D. J., McInerney, P. A., Godfrey, C. M., Alexander, L., Evans, C., Pieper, D., Moraes, E. B., Tricco, A. C., Munn, Z., & Pollock, D. (2022). The role of scoping reviews in reducing research waste. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, S0895435622002335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2022.09.012

  • Krems, J. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2022). Updating long-held assumptions about fat stigma: For women, body shape plays a critical role. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(1), 70–82. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550621991381

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Kret, M. E., Pichon, S., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2011). Similarities and differences in perceiving threat from dynamic faces and bodies. An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1755–1762.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.012

  • *Kruse, B., Bogler, C., Haynes, J.-D., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2016). Am I seeing myself, my friend or a stranger? The role of personal familiarity in visual distinction of body identities in the human brain. Cortex, 83, 86–100.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.07.010

  • *Kurdi, B., Lozano, S., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Introducing the open affective standardized image set (OASIS). Behavior Research Methods49, 457–470.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0715-3

  • *Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. University of Florida.

  • Langner, O., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., Wigboldus, D. H., Hawk, S. T., & Van Knippenberg, A. D. (2010). Presentation and validation of the radboud faces database. Cognition and Emotion, 24(8), 1377–1388. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903485076

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leehr, E., Giel, K. E., Schaeffeler, N., Mack, I., Thiel, A., Zurstiege, G., & Zipfel, S. (2018). Where do you look? Visual attention to human bodies across the weight spectrum in individuals with normal weight or with obesity. Obesity Facts, 11(4), 277–286. https://doi.org/10.1159/000489787

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leonelli, S. (2023). Philosophy of open science. Cambridge.

  • Lin, Ma, Q., Huang, X., Wu, X., & Zhang, Y. (2023). Pervasive failure to report essential stimulus properties in experimental research in psychology and neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/hy58r

  • *Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & Burriss, R. P. (2007). Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 51(5), 633–639.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.03.006

  • Loper, M., Mahmood, N., Romero, J., Pons-moll, G., & Black, M. J. (2015). SMPL: A skinned multi-person linear model. ACM Transactions on Graphics, 34(6), Article 248. https://doi.org/10.1145/2816795.2818013

  • Ma, D. S., Correll, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2015). The Chicago face database: A free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1122–1135. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0532-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, D. S., Kantner, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2021). Chicago face database: Multiracial expansion. Behavior Research Methods, 53, 1289–1300. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01482-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Marchewka, A., Żurawski, Ł., Jednoróg, K., & Grabowska, A. (2014). The Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS): Introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. Behavior Research Methods46, 596–610.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0379-1

  • Matsick, J. L., Kruk, M., Oswald, F., & Palmer, L. (2021). Bridging feminist psychology and open science: Feminist tools and shared values inform best practices for science reform. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 45(4), 412–429. https://doi.org/10.1177/03616843211026564

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Max Planck Institute. (2011). Body Shape Visualizer. [Software]. https://bodyvisualizer.com

  • McElvaney, T. J., Osman, M., & Mareschal, I. (2021). Perceiving threat in others: The role of body morphology. PLoS ONE, 16(4), e0249782. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249782

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Minnebusch, D. A., Keune, P. M., Suchan, B., & Daum, I. (2010). Gradual inversion affects the processing of human body shapes. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2746–2755.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.046

  • *Mishra, S., Srinivasan, N., Shanker, U., & Asif, M. (2021). Affective Film Dataset from India (AFDI): Creation and validation with an Indian Sample. Advance online publication (preprint). https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qjvpz

  • Mohr, H. M., Rickmeyer, C., Hummel, D., Ernst, M., & Grabhorn, R. (2016). Altered visual adaptation to body shape in eating disorders: Implications for body image distortion. Perception, 45(7), 725–738. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616633385

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. (1988). Impressions of people created by age-related qualities of their gaits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(4), 547–556. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.547

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Munn, Z., Peters, M. D. J., Stern, C., Tufanaru, C., McArthur, A., & Aromataris, E. (2018). Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 18(1), 143. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-018-0611-x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Munn, Z., Pollock, D., Khalil, H., Alexander, L., Mclnerney, P., Godfrey, C. M., Peters, M., & Tricco, A. C. (2022). What are scoping reviews? Providing a formal definition of scoping reviews as a type of evidence synthesis. JBI Evidence Synthesis, 20(4), 950–952. https://doi.org/10.11124/JBIES-21-00483

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science349(6251), aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716

  • *Orgs, G., Bestmann, S., Schuur, F., & Haggard, P. (2011). From body form to biological motion: The apparent velocity of human movement biases subjective time. Psychological Science, 22(6), 712–717.https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611406446

  • Oswald, F. (2023). Perception, experience, and body identity in intergroup relations: How body size shapes visual subjects and perceivers. [Doctoral Dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University]. https://etda.libraries.psu.edu/catalog/21269feo5020

  • Oswald, F., & Adams Jr, R. B. (2022). Feminist social vision: Seeing through the lens of marginalized perceivers. Personality and Social Psychology Review. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683221126582

  • Oswald, F., Champion, A., Khera, D., Mitchell, J., & Pedersen, C. L. (2021). Bisexual stereotypes apply differently by body size: An assessment of bisexual prototypicality, trait application, and body size. Journal of Bisexuality, 21(4), 484–515. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299716.2021.1994905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, F., Champion, A., & Pedersen, C. L. (2022a). The influence of body shape on impressions of sexual traits. The Journal of Sex Research, 59(3), 330–343. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2020.1841723

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, F., Champion, A., Khera, D., Young, M., & Pedersen, C. L. (2022b). Stereotype application at the intersection of body shape, gender/sex, and sexual orientation. The Journal of Sex Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2022.2026286

  • Oswald, F., Griffin, J. W., Weisbuch, M., & Adams, R. B. (2023). People watching: Social perception and the ensemble coding of bodies. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 47, 545–568. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00443-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Toole, A. J., Ha, J., Snow, S. L., Hurst, D. R., Pappas, M. R., Ayyad, J. H., & Abdi, H. (2005). A video database of moving faces and people. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 27(5), 812–816. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2005.90

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overney, L. S., Michel, C. M., Harris, I. M., & Pegna, A. J. (2005). Cerebral processes in mental transformations of body parts: Recognition prior to rotation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 722–734. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.024

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paganini, G. A., McConnell, A. A., Deska, J. C., Almaraz, S. M., Hugenberg, K., & Lloyd, E. P. (2023). Waist-to-hip ratio predicts sexual perception and responses to sexual assault disclosures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167222114

  • Peters, M. D. J., Godfrey, C., McInerney, P., Khalil, H., Larsen, P., Marnie, C., Pollock, D., Tricco, A. C., & Munn, Z. (2022). Best practice guidance and reporting items for the development of scoping review protocols. JBI Evidence Synthesis, 20(4), 953–968. https://doi.org/10.11124/JBIES-21-00242

  • Peters, M. D. J., Marnie, C., Tricco, A. C., Pollock, D., Munn, Z., Alexander, L., McInerney, P., Godfrey, C. M., & Khalil, H. (2020). Updated methodological guidance for the conduct of scoping reviews. JBI Evidence Synthesis, 18(10), 2119–2126. https://doi.org/10.11124/JBIES-20-00167

  • *Pietsch, S., & Jansen, P. (2018). Mental rotation and handedness: Differences in object-based and egocentric transformations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 30(5–6), 511–519.https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2018.1483937

  • *Pietsch, S., Jansen, P., & Lehmann, J. (2019). The choice of sports affects mental rotation performance in adolescents. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 224.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00224

  • *Press, S. A., Biehl, S. C., Vatheuer, C. C., Domes, G., & Svaldi, J. (2022). Neural correlates of body image processing in binge eating disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 131(4), 350–364.https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000750

  • Princeton University. (n.d.). Face image directory. https://libguides.princeton.edu/facedatabases

  • Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 11401–11405. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721165115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, R. (2018). Neural integration in body perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(10), 1442–1451. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01299

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Reed, C. L., & Farah, M. J. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(2), 334–343.https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.2.334

  • Reed, C. L., Beall, P. M., Stone, V. E., Kopelioff, L., Pulham, D. J., & Hepburn, S. L. (2007). Brief report: Perception of body posture—what individuals with autism spectrum disorder might be missing. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1576–1584. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0220-0

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, E., & Kirkham, T. C. (2014). Is he a healthy weight? Exposure to obesity changes perception of the weight status of others. International Journal of Obesity, 38(5), 663–667. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2013.154

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2020). A meta-psychological perspective on the decade of replication failures in social psychology. Canadian Psychology / Psychologie Canadienne, 61(4), 364–376. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Shimada, S. (2013). Localization of extrastriate body area using functional near-infrared spectroscopy and 3D digitizer. 2013 35th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 7408–7411. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2013.6611270

  • Shropshire, J. L., & Johnson, K. L. (2021). Harnessing visible representation to mitigate bias. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8(1), 27–33. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732220984800

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Sinke, C. B. A., Sorger, B., Goebel, R., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Tease or threat? Judging social interactions from bodily expressions. NeuroImage, 49(2), 1717–1727.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.065

  • *Skinner, A., Bould, H., Andy, Etchells, P. J., Penton-Voak, I., Clark, A., Harmer, C., Park, R., Broome, M., & Munafo, M. R. (2017). The Morphed Photographic Figure Scale: Creation and validation of a novel set of realistic female body stimuli [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9h73c

  • Slaughter, V., Stone, V. E., & Reed, C. (2004). Perception of faces and bodies: Similar or different? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(6), 219–223. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00312.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Steggemann, Y., Engbert, K., & Weigelt, M. (2011). Selective effects of motor expertise in mental body rotation tasks: Comparing object-based and perspective transformations. Brain and Cognition, 76(1), 97–105.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.02.013

  • *Stephen, I. D., Bickersteth, C., Mond, J., Stevenson, R. J., & Brooks, K. R. (2016). No effect of featural attention on body size aftereffects. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01223

  • Strohminger, N., Gray, K., Chituc, V., Heffner, J., Schein, C., & Heagins, T. B. (2016). The MR2: a multi-racial, mega-resolution database of facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 1197–1204. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0641-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taubert, N., Li, J., Endres, D., & Giese, M. (2015). Dependence of the perception of emotional body movements on concurrent social motor behavior. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 505–505. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.12.505

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Taylor, J. C., Roberts, M. V., Downing, P. E., & Thierry, G. (2010). Functional characterisation of the extrastriate body area based on the N1 ERP component. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 153–159.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.04.001

  • *Thierry, G., Pegna, A. J., Dodds, C., Roberts, M., Basan, S., & Downing, P. (2006). An event-related potential component sensitive to images of the human body. NeuroImage, 32(2), 871–879.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.060

  • *Thoma, P., Bauser, D. S., & Suchan, B. (2013). BESST (Bochum Emotional Stimulus Set)—A pilot validation study of a stimulus set containing emotional bodies and faces from frontal and averted views. Psychiatry Research209(1), 98–109.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.012

  • *Tovée, M. J., Maisey, D. S., Emery, J. L., & Cornelissen, P. L. (1999). Visual cues to female physical attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266(1415), 211–218. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1999.0624

  • Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O’Brien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., Moher, D., Peters, M. D. J., Horsley, T., Weeks, L., Hempel, S., Akl, E. A., Chang, C., McGowan, J., Stewart, L., Hartling, L., Aldcroft, A., Wilson, M. G., Garritty, C., … Straus, S. E. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and explanation. Annals of Internal Medicine, 169(7), 467–473. https://doi.org/10.7326/M18-0850

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Urgesi, C., Calvo-Merino, B., Haggard, P., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals two cortical pathways for visual body processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(30), 8023–8030.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0789-07.2007

  • *Urgesi, C., Fornasari, L., Canalaz, F., Perini, L., Cremaschi, S., Faleschini, L., Thyrion, E. Z., Zuliani, M., Balestrieri, M., Fabbro, F., & Brambilla, P. (2014). Impaired configural body processing in anorexia nervosa: Evidence from the body inversion effect. British Journal of Psychology, 105(4), 486–508.https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12057

  • *Uusberg, H., Peet, K., Uusberg, A., & Akkermann, K. (2018). Attention biases in preoccupation with body image: An ERP study of the role of social comparison and automaticity when processing body size. Biological Psychology, 135, 136–148.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.03.007

  • Van den Akker, O., Peters, G.-J. Y., Bakker, C., Carlsson, R., Coles, N. A., Corker, K. S., Feldman, G., Mellor, D. T., Moreau, D., Nordström, T., Pfeiffer, N., Pickering, J. S., Riegelman, A., Topor, M., van Veggel, N., & Yeung, S. K. (2020). Inclusive Systematic Review Registration Form. MetaArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31222/osf.io/3nbea

  • Verfaillie, K., & Daems, A. (2002). Representing and anticipating human actions in vision. Visual Cognition, 9(1–2), 217–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280143000403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Voges, M. M., Giabbiconi, C.-M., Gruber, T., Andersen, S. K., Hartmann, A. S., & Vocks, S. (2019). Sustained hypervigilance for one’s own body in women with weight and shape concerns: Competition effects in early visual processing investigated by steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEP). Biological Psychology, 143, 74–84.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.02.010

  • Wallace, B. C., Small, K., Brodley, C. E., Lau, J., & Trikalinos, T. A. (2012, January). Deploying an interactive machine learning system in an evidence-based practice center: abstrackr. In Proceedings of the 2nd ACM SIGHIT International Health Informatics Symposium (pp. 819–824).

  • *Walsh, J., Eccleston, C., & Keogh, E. (2014). Pain communication through body posture: The development and validation of a stimulus set. Pain, 155(11), 2282–2290.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.08.019

  • Wilson, J. P., Hugenberg, K., & Rule, N. O. (2017). Racial bias in judgments of physical size and formidability: From size to threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(1), 59–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000092

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Winkler, C., & Rhodes, G. (2005). Perceptual adaptation affects attractiveness of female bodies. British Journal of Psychology, 96(2), 141–154.https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X36343

  • *Withnell, S., Sears, C. R., & von Ranson, K. M. (2019). How malleable are attentional biases in women with body dissatisfaction? Priming effects and their impact on attention to images of women’s bodies. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 10(2), 204380871983713.https://doi.org/10.1177/2043808719837137

  • *Zentgraf, K., Stark, R., Reiser, M., Künzell, S., Schienle, A., Kirsch, P., Walter, B., Vaitl, D., & Munzert, J. (2005). Differential activation of pre-SMA and SMA proper during action observation: Effects of instructions. NeuroImage, 26(3), 662–672.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.015

  • *Zimmermann, M., Mars, R. B., de Lange, F. P., Toni, I., & Verhagen, L. (2018). Is the extrastriate body area part of the dorsal visuomotor stream? Brain Structure and Function, 223(1), 31–46.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1469-0

Download references

Funding

The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Flora Oswald.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

Not applicable.

Consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Conflict of interest/Competing interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Additional information

Open practices statement

Data and materials included in the scoping review are available at: https://osf.io/mz54e/?view_only=754ad2a7646c4dbebc3b709e9044b3f4

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 107 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Oswald, F., Samra, S.K. A scoping review and index of body stimuli in psychological science. Behav Res (2023). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02278-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02278-z

Keywords

Navigation