Skip to main content
Log in

Incidentally encoded temporal associations produce priming in implicit memory

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A key assumption of some leading memory theories is that information about the relative order of events is automatically encoded whenever memories are formed and automatically retrieved whenever events are remembered. This information is often used to guide memory search: Remembering one event tends to trigger the retrieval of other events previously experienced nearby in time (temporal contiguity effect). The retrieved context framework attributes this temporal contiguity effect to automatic encoding and retrieval processes, predicting temporal contiguity even in incidental encoding and implicit retrieval. There is strong evidence of temporal contiguity following incidental encoding, but does the prediction hold for implicit retrieval? In this experiment, we tested the framework’s predictions for recall and repetition priming. Across 30 trials, undergraduates (\(n = 603\)) read a series of words aloud as they appeared onscreen. In each trial, two words were repeated (cue and target), initially separated by |lag\(| =\) 1, 2, or 5. On their second presentation, the cue word was presented first, immediately followed by the target word. We found a strong temporal contiguity effect in a surprise free recall test, replicating previous work with explicit retrieval. For implicit retrieval, we compared repetition priming (how quickly subjects began reading a word on its first versus second presentation) for cue and target words. Repeating a cue word enhanced repetition priming for its associated target word, and this effect varied with the initial lag between the cue and target. These results support theories that assume temporal information is encoded and retrieved automatically.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Open practices statement

De-identified data and data analysis scripts are available at: https://osf.io/392cs/?view_only=b7610fcf7d86422b87393107c1f28927.

Notes

  1. We considered three other approaches to exclusions. In the first approach, we excluded extreme outliers (as defined above), and then any value more than 2.5 standard deviations from the mean for each subject was replaced with a value equal to the subject’s mean response time plus 2.5 standard deviations (if the original value was above the mean) or minus 2.5 standard deviations (if the original value was below the mean). The second approach was to make exclusions based on the mean and standard deviation across subjects for each item type, rather than the mean and standard deviation for each subject. The third method was to make no exclusions at all. None of these methods changed the direction or significance of our results.

  2. For details, see Supplementary Materials.

  3. We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out the potential confound between inter-presentation lag and item type and suggesting a supplementary analysis.

  4. The by-lag analyses are based on the 602 subjects who contributed data for all 6 possible lags.

  5. Temporal factor scores cannot be calculated for subjects who did not make at least one valid transition, so those subjects were excluded from this analysis.

References

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Linh Lazarus for helpful discussions.

Funding

This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 1848972.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Abigail M. D. Mundorf or M. Karl Healey.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

The study procedures and materials were approved by the Michigan State University Institutional Review Board. All elements of this study were performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual subjects included in the study.

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file 1 (pdf 1019 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mundorf, A.M.D., Uitvlugt, M.G. & Healey, M.K. Incidentally encoded temporal associations produce priming in implicit memory. Psychon Bull Rev 31, 761–771 (2024). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-023-02351-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-023-02351-w

Keywords

Navigation