Andersen, H. (1973). Abductive and deductive change. Language, 49(4), 765–793.
Article
Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., & Bickerton, D. (2010). The emergence of protolanguage: Holophrasis vs compositionality (Vol. 24). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Google Scholar
Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Sandler, W. (2005). The paradox of sign language morphology. Language, 81(2), 301–344.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: An experimental and social study. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University.
Google Scholar
Batali, J. (1998). Computational simulations of the emergence of grammar. In J. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases (pp. 405-426). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Batali, J. (2002). The negotiation and acquisition of recursive grammars as a result of competition among exemplars. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition (pp. 111–172). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Belyaev, D. K. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. The Journal of Heredity, 70(5), 301–308.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us: Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Okanoya, K., Beckers, G. J. L., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2011). Songs to syntax: The linguistics of birdsong. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 113–121.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Brace, L., Bullock, S., & Noble, J. (2015). Achieving compositional language in a population of iterated learners. In P. Andrews, L. Caves, R. Doursat, S. Hickinbotham, F. Polack, S. Stepney, . . . J. Timmis (Eds.), Advances in artificial life, ECAL 2015 (pp. 349–356). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Brighton, H. (2002). Compositional syntax from cultural transmission. Artificial Life, 8(1), 25–54.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Brighton, H., Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2005). Language as an evolutionary system. Physics of Life Reviews, 2(3), 177–226.
Article
Google Scholar
Burkett, D., & Griffiths, T. L. (2010). Iterated learning of multiple languages from multiple teachers. In A.D.M. Smith, M. Schouwstra, B. de Boer & K. Smith (Eds.), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 8th International Conference (pp. 58–65). Singapore: World Scientific.
Carr, J., Smith, K., Cornish, H., & Kirby, S. (2016). The cultural evolution of structured languages in an open-ended, continuous world. Cognitive Science. doi:10.1111/cogs.12371
Carstensen, A., Xu, J., Smith, C. T., & Regier, T. (2015). Language evolution in the lab tends toward informative communication. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, July 2015 (pp. 303-308). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2010). Language acquisition meets language evolution. Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1131–1157.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Chater, N., Reali, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2009). Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(4), 1015–1020.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Chater, N., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Yuille, A. (2006). Probabilistic models of cognition: Conceptual foundations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(7), 287–291.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Chater, N., & Vitányi, P. (2003). Simplicity: A unifying principle in cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 19–22.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Christensen, P., Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K. (2016). Environmental constraints shaping constituent order in emerging communication systems: Structural iconicity, interactive alignment and conventionalization. Cognition, 146, 67–80.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Claidiere, N., Smith, K., Kirby, S., & Fagot, J. (2014). Cultural evolution of systematically structured behaviour in a non-human primate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1797), 20141541–20141541.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Clay, Z., Pople, S., Hood, B., & Kita, S. (2014). Young children make their gestural communication systems more language-like: Segmentation and linearization of semantic elements in motion events. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1518–1525.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cornish, H., Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2013). Systems from sequences: An iterated learning account of the emergence of systematic structure in a non-linguistic task. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, N. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 340–345). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Culbertson, J., & Adger, D. (2014). Language learners privilege structured meaning over surface frequency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(16), 5842–5847.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Culbertson, J., & Kirby, S. (2015). Simplicity and specificity in language: Domain-general biases have domain-specific effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1964.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Culbertson, J., Smolensky, P., & Legendre, G. (2012). Learning biases predict a word order universal. Cognition, 122(3), 306–329.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London, UK: John Murray.
Book
Google Scholar
de Vos, C. (2015). The Kata Kolok pointing system: Morphemization and syntactic integration. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(1), 150–168.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (2003). Multilevel selection in a complex adaptive system: The problem of language origins. In B. H. Weber & D. J. Depew (Eds.), Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered (pp. 81–106). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (2009). Relaxed selection and the role of epigenesis in the evolution of language. In M. S. Blumberg, J. H. Freeman, & S. R. Robinson (Eds.), Oxford handbook of development behavioral neuroscience (pp. 730–752). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (2010). A role for relaxed selection in the evolution of the language capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(Suppl. 2), 9000–9006.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fay, N., Garrod, S., Roberts, L., & Swoboda, N. (2010). The interactive evolution of human communication systems. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 351–386.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fehér, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P., & Tchernichovski, O. (2009). De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature, 459(7246), 564–568.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition, 100(1), 173–215.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Frank, M. C., & Goodman, N. D. (2012). Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science, 336(6084), 998.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Galantucci, B. (2005). An experimental study of the emergence of human communication systems. Cognitive Science, 29(5), 737–767.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (2004). Conceptual spaces: The geometry of thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Garrod, S., Fay, N., Lee, J., Oberlander, J., & Macleod, T. (2007). Foundations of representation: Where might graphical symbol systems come from? Cognitive Science, 31(6), 961–987.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gibson, E., & Wexler, K. (1994). Triggers. Linguistic Inquiry, 25(3), 407–454.
Google Scholar
Gold, E. M. (1967). Language identification in the limit. Information and Control, 10(5), 447–474.
Article
Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Brentari, D. (2015). Gesture, sign and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–82. doi:10.1017/S0140525X15001247
Goldin-Meadow, S., So, W. C., Ozyürek, A., & Mylander, C. (2008). The natural order of events: How speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(27), 9163–9168.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., & Kalish, M. L. (2007). Language evolution by iterated learning with Bayesian agents. Cognitive Science, 31(3), 441–480.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R., & Trut, L. (2005). Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Current Biology, 15(3), 226–230.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hare, B., Wobber, V., & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83(3), 573–585.
Article
Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2007). Dual-inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 555–570). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203, 89–96.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2000). Social transmission favours linguistic generalization. In C. Knight & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form (pp. 324–352). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2002). Expression/induction models of language evolution: Dimensions and issues. In E. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models (pp. 301-344). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1999). Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(7), 272–279.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kalish, M. L., Griffiths, T. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2007). Iterated learning: Intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 288–294.
Article
Google Scholar
Keller, R. (1994). On language change: The invisible hand in language. London, UK: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Kemp, C., & Regier, T. (2012). Kinship categories across languages reflect general communicative principles. Science, 336(6084), 1049–1054.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kirby, S. (1999). Function, selection, and innateness: The emergence of language universals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Kirby, S. (2000). Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In C. Knight, J. Hurford, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), The emergence of language (pp. 303–323). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Kirby, S. (2001). Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure-an iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 5(2), 102–110.
Article
Google Scholar
Kirby, S. (2002). Learning, bottlenecks and the evolution of recursive syntax. In E. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition (pp. 173-204). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(31), 10681–10686.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kirby, S., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(12), 5241–5245.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kirby, S., Griffiths, T., & Smith, K. (2014). Iterated learning and the evolution of language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 108–114.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kirby, S., & Hurford, J. (1997). Learning, culture and evolution in the origin of linguistic constraints. In P. Husbands & I. Harvey (Eds.), Fourth European Conference on Artificial Life (pp. 493–502). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kirby, S., & Hurford, J. R. (2002). The emergence of linguistic structure: An overview of the iterated learning model. In A. Cangelosi & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the Evolution of Language (pp. 121–147). London, UK: Springer.
Kirby, S., Tamariz, M., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2015). Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition, 141, 87–102.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Krifka, M. (2001). Compositionality. In R. A. Wilson & F. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopaedia of the cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Maynard Smith, J., & Szathmary, E. (1995). The major transitions of life. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Google Scholar
Meir, I., Israel, A., Sandler, W., Padden, C. A., & Aronoff, M. (2012). The influence of community on language structure: Evidence from two young sign languages. Linguistic Variation, 12(2), 247–291.
Article
Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3489–3501.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., Google Books Team, Pickett, J. P., Hoiberg, D., Clancy, D., Norvig, P., Orwant, J., Pinker, S., Nowak, M. A. & Lierberman Aiden, E. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331(6014), 176–182.
Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. L., & Niyogi, P. (2002). Computational and evolutionary aspects of language. Nature, 417(6889), 611–617.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Nowicki, S., Peters, S., & Podos, J. (1998). Song learning, early nutrition and sexual selection in songbirds. American Zoologist, 38(1), 179–190.
Article
Google Scholar
Nyst, V. A. S. (2007). A descriptive analysis of Adamorobe sign language (Ghana). Utrecht, The Netherlands: LOT Publications.
Google Scholar
Okanoya, K. (2002). Sexual display as a syntactical vehicle: The evolution of syntax in birdsong and human language through sexual selection. In A. Wray (Ed.), The Transition to Language (pp. 46-63). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Okanoya, K. (2004). The Bengalese finch: A window on the behavioral neurobiology of birdsong syntax. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016, 724–735.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Okanoya, K. (2015). Evolution of song complexity in Bengalese finches: Sexual selection and domestication as two factors. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(3), 1880–1880.
Article
Google Scholar
Perfors, A., & Navarro, D. J. (2014). Language evolution can be shaped by the structure of the world. Cognitive Science, 38(4), 775–793.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Perfors, A., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Regier, T. (2011). The learnability of abstract syntactic principles. Cognition, 118(3), 306–338.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(04), 707-727.
Ravignani, A., Delgado, T., & Kirby, S. (2016). Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nature Human Behaviour, in press.
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6(6), 855–863.
Article
Google Scholar
Ritchie, G. R. S., Kirby, S., & Hawkey, D. J. C. (2008). Song learning as an indicator mechanism: Modelling the developmental stress hypothesis. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 251(4), 570–583.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Savage, P. E., Brown, S., Sakai, E., & Currie, T. E. (2015). Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(29), 8987–8992.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Schouwstra, M., & de Swart, H. (2014). The semantic origins of word order. Cognition, 131(3), 431–436.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. (2014). Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. London, UK: Palgrave MacMillan.
Google Scholar
Senghas, A., Kita, S., & Ozyürek, A. (2004). Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science, 305(5691), 1779–1782.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28(4), 1070–1094.
Article
Google Scholar
Smith, K. (2009). Iterated learning in populations of Bayesian agents. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 697–702). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Smith, A. D. M. (2014). Models of language evolution and change. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(3), 281–293.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, K., Abramova, K., Cartmill, E., & Kirby, S. (2016). The experimental study of sign language emergence. Manuscript in preparation.
Smith, K., Brighton, H., & Kirby, S. (2003). Complex systems in language evolution: The cultural emergence of compositional structure. Advances in Complex Systems: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 06(04), 537–558.
Article
Google Scholar
Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2008). Cultural evolution: Implications for understanding the human language faculty and its evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3591–3603.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2012). Compositionality and linguistic evolution. In W. Hinzen, E. Machery, & M. Werning (Eds.), Oxford handbook of compositionality (pp. 493-509). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Smith, K., Kirby, S., & Brighton, H. (2003). Iterated learning: A framework for the emergence of language. Artificial Life, 9(4), 371–386.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, K., & Wonnacott, E. (2010). Eliminating unpredictable variation through iterated learning. Cognition, 116(3), 444–449.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Tamariz, M., & Kirby, S. (2015). Culture: Copying, compression, and conventionality. Cognitive Science, 39(1), 171–183.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Thomas, J. (2014). Self-domestication and language evolution (Doctoral thesis, University of Edinburgh, Scotland).
Thompson, B. (2015). Transmission, induction and evolution (Doctoral thesis, University of Edinburgh, Scotland).
Thompson, B., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2016). Culture shapes the evolution of cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 113(16), 4530–4535.
Article
Google Scholar
Trut, L. (1999). Early canid domestication: The farm-fox experiment: Foxes bred for tamability in a 40-year experiment exhibit remarkable transformations that suggest an interplay between behavioral genetics and development. American Scientist, 87(2), 160–169.
Article
Google Scholar
Verhoef, T. (2012). The origins of duality of patterning in artificial whistled languages. Language and Cognition, 4(4), 357–380.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wedel, A. B. (2012). Lexical contrast maintenance and the organization of sublexical contrast systems. Language and Cognition, 4, 319–355.
Article
Google Scholar
Werning, M., Hinzen, W., & Machery, E. (2012). The Oxford handbook of compositionality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Winters, J., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2015). Languages adapt to their contextual niche. Language and Cognition, 7(03), 415–449.
Article
Google Scholar
Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language & Communication, 18(1), 47–67.
Article
Google Scholar
Xu, J., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. L. (2013). Cultural transmission results in convergence towards colour term universals. Proceedings: Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 280(1758), 20123073.
Google Scholar
Xu, Y., & Regier, T. (2014). Numeral systems across languages support efficient communication: From approximate numerosity to recursion. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1802-1807). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Zuidema, W. (2003). How the poverty of the stimulus solves the poverty of the stimulus. In, S. Becker, S. Thrun & K. Obermayer (Eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 15 (pp. 51–58). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.