Many activities in our personal and professional lives depend on our ability to remember to perform deferred intended actions when certain events or stimuli are encountered. This type of task is referred to as event-based prospective memory (PM). The most widely used event-based PM paradigm, introduced by Einstein and McDaniel (1990), requires participants to remember to perform an atypical task action (e.g., press the F1 key) when presented with a target event (e.g., a particular syllable) while engaged in an ongoing task (e.g., categorization). Much of the PM literature has been concerned with delineating the cognitive mechanisms by which individuals remember to respond to targets with the PM response.

Numerous studies have demonstrated that response times to ongoing tasks on nontarget trials are slowed when individuals need to remember to perform PM tasks, an effect referred to as the costs to ongoing tasks (for a review, see Smith, Hunt, McVay, & McConnell, 2007). Costs have been interpreted as evidence for the allocation of some form of attentional capacity to the PM task and have formed the primary dependent measure for determining the resource requirements of PM (Einstein & McDaniel, 2010; Smith, 2010). Savine, McDaniel, Shelton, and Scullin (2012) recently demonstrated differences in how individuals temporally distribute attention to PM tasks. We asked a related but different question: Can we find direct evidence that participants process individual ongoing task items differently when they have PM requirements, as compared with when they do not? We answered this research question by examining the recognition of nontarget words previously presented in ongoing tasks with PM requirements.

There have been several theoretical proposals regarding the control processes allocated to PM tasks. According to the Smith (2003) preparatory attentional and memory processes (PAM) theory, individuals allocate “preparatory attention” to prepare for a switch between ongoing task and PM task responses. Similarly, Guynn (2003) proposed that individuals maintain their memory system in a retrieval mode, described as a mental set for treating ongoing task items as PM retrieval cues. Participants may also rehearse PM instructions (Smith, 2010). These generic PM processes can be distinguished from PM processes directed at individual ongoing task items. Guynn (2003) and the multiprocess view of PM (Einstein & McDaniel, 2005) propose that participants periodically “check” or “monitor” for targets. In line with this, PAM proposes that preparatory attention serves the additional function of mapping individual ongoing task stimuli onto PM intentions (also see Savine et al., 2012; “attentional focus model”).

Costs to nontarget ongoing task trials cannot be used to directly infer that individual ongoing task items have been differentially processed for PM, since these costs may also reflect generic PM processes. Findings of increased response time to PM lures demonstrate that ongoing task items can receive extra individual attention if their stimulus properties partially or exactly match PM targets (Knight, Meeks, Marsh, Cook, Brewer & Hicks, 2011). PM goals are likely to be spontaneously retrieved when PM lures are processed, with the increased response time caused either by this initial noticing or by subsequent target verification, intention retrieval, or response coordination (Knight et al., 2011). In contrast, the present study is concerned with whether participants differentially process nontarget items that have not previously attracted attention because of their relationship to the conditions needed to satisfy the PM task. Participants performed a lexical decision task, either with or without PM requirements, followed by a surprise recognition memory task, where nontarget words from the lexical decision task were intermixed with new words. The recognition of nontargets previously presented in the ongoing task was expected to provide a measure of the differential processing of nontarget ongoing task items (Burt, Walker, Humphreys, & Tehan, 1993; Humphreys, Maguire, McFarlane, Burt, Bolland, Murray & Dunn, 2010).

Participants in the control condition performed the lexical decision task without a PM task. Some participants were required to make the PM response when presented with a specific word. Detecting a specific target is considered focal to lexical decision because lexical decision requires participants to decide whether letter strings are words or not. Other participants made the PM response when presented with a category exemplar. Detecting category exemplars is nonfocal because lexical decision does not require individuals to process the semantic features of letter strings necessary to make a category determination (Einstein & McDaniel, 2005). PM performance is typically higher for specific (focal) than for categorical (nonfocal) targets (Marsh, Hicks, Cook, Hansen, & Pallos, 2003).

We expected to replicate previous findings of costs to ongoing tasks for categorical (nonfocal) PM conditions, as compared with controls. Generic PM processes underlying these costs should not benefit recognition. In contrast, specific PM processes that operate on individual ongoing task items may improve recognition. Specifically, if participants in the nonfocal condition map the semantic features of letter strings onto the semantic features of their PM category, this elaboration in processing may increase the incidental learning of nontargets over and above that provided by making lexical decisions, enhancing the subsequent recognition of those nontargets (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). There are typically no costs to lexical decisions for focal PM conditions (for an exception, see Smith, 2010). Numerous studies demonstrate that focal PM tasks can be spontaneously retrieved without the need to maintain PM cognitive control processes on nontarget trials (e.g., Harrison & Einstein, 2010). We included a second focal condition where the importance of the PM task was emphasized (Loft & Yeo, 2007). We expected that the focal-importance condition would show costs of similar size as the nonfocal condition due to the employment of a variety of possible general or specific cognitive control processes. However, there is still no logical necessity to retrieve semantic information in order to determine whether a letter string being processed corresponds to a specific target word. Thus, there is no basis to assume that the important focal PM task will enhance recognition. Inclusion of the focal-importance condition does, however, provide an experimental demonstration that the improved recognition of nontargets in the nonfocal condition is not an epiphenomenon of prolonged presentation (study) time during lexical decision. Finally, for completeness, a second nonfocal condition was included where the importance of the PM task was emphasized.

Method

Participants

Two hundred eighty-eight undergraduates participated and were randomly assigned to conditions. There were 96 participants in the control condition, and 48 participants in each of the four PM conditions.

Materials

A pool of 384 medium frequency words (occurring 20–50 times per million) were randomly selected from the Sydney Morning Herald word database (Dennis, 1995). Of these, 184 were used as words, and 200 were converted to nonwords by randomly replacing each vowel in each word (e.g., chemist to chamust).

Two lists were created, and each list contained 92 words and 100 nonwords. Each list had a designated category of PM targets embedded within it (sports or fruits). Eight medium-typicality exemplars from each category were used as targets (Van Overschelde, Rawson, & Dunlosky, 2004). In list A, the category was sport (golf, softball, soccer, cricket, hockey, lacrosse, tennis, rugby). In list B, the category was fruit (grape, pear, peach, melon, cherry, banana, plum, mango). Words and nonwords from one list were used as stimuli for lexical decision, and a random subset of nontarget words from the other list were used as new items in the recognition task. The assignment of lists to the lexical decision and recognition task was counterbalanced.

For the lexical decision task, the presentation of words and nonwords within each list was random, except that targets were presented in a random position between trials 5 and 25, 26 and 50, 51 and 75, 76 and 100, 101 and 125, 126 and 150, 151 and 175, and 176 and 200. The focal conditions were presented with one category exemplar eight times, one at each allocated target trial position. Each exemplar of each category was used equally as often as a focal target across participants. In the nonfocal conditions, one category exemplar was presented in each allocated target trial position. Overall, in the nonfocal conditions, eight different category exemplars were presented. In the control condition, targets were also presented, but the targets held no special significance. Half the control condition we presented with one focal target, and half with category exemplars.

The recognition task presented 80 of the old words previously presented in the lexical decision task and 40 randomly selected new words from the alternative list. Old words were excluded from the recognition task if they were presented on the first two trials of the lexical decision task, were targets, or were presented in one of the two trials following targets. The recognition task consisted of four blocks of 30 words (20 old and 10 new). In order to approximately equate the average retention interval across items (Humphreys et al., 2010), the first 20 valid words presented in the lexical decision task were presented in the first recognition block, the next 20 lexical decision valid words were presented in the second block, and so on for the third and fourth blocks. The assignment of new words to the recognition task blocks was random, as was the presentation order of new and old words within each block.

Procedure

For lexical decision, participants were instructed to decide whether letter strings were English words or nonwords and to respond by pressing “f” for a word or “j” for a nonword. The first display was a fixation point “+” displayed in white on a black background for 500 ms. The fixation point was then replaced by a blank screen for 250 ms, followed by the presentation of the letter string, which remained on the screen until the participant made a response.

Participants were told to respond as quickly and accurately as possible to the lexical decision task. Participants in the PM conditions were additionally instructed to press the “9” key when presented with a target during the ongoing task and were told that they could press the “9” key on one of the next two trials if they forgot to on the target trial. Participants in the focal conditions were instructed to press “9” whenever a specific target word was presented. Participants in the nonfocal conditions were instructed to press “9” when an exemplar of a target category was presented. The participants in the PM important conditions were additionally instructed that it was important that they remember to make the PM response. Participants completed a 3-min distractor puzzle before beginning lexical decision. Following the lexical decision task, participants completed a second 3-min distractor puzzle before performing the recognition task. On the recognition task, participants were presented with a 500-ms blank screen, followed by a single word. Participants pressed “o” if they believed that the word had been presented in the lexical decision task and “n” if they believed that the word had not appeared in the lexical decision task.

Results

PM performance, response time to the lexical decision task, and hit rates, false alarm rates, and discrimination (d) for the recognition task are presented in Table 1. First, we conducted 2 (focality) × 2 (importance) between-subjects ANOVAs to confirm that the focality and importance manipulations affected PM and the ongoing task in a manner consistent with past research. We then conducted planned contrasts that directly paralleled our hypotheses (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1985), by comparing the ongoing task response time and recognition task performance for each experimental condition with those in the control condition.

Table 1 Prospective memory accuracy (PM accuracy), lexical decision response time (LD RT), and hit rate, false alarm rate, and discrimination (d′) on the recognition task as a function of condition (with standard deviations in parentheses)

Prospective memory

PM responses were scored as correct if participants pressed the “9” key on the target trial or on the following two trials. There were main effects of focality, F(1, 188) = 50.60, p < .001, η 2p = .21, and importance, F(1, 188) = 19.47, p < .001, η 2p = .09, but no interaction, F(1, 188) = 1.59, p = .21, η 2p = .01. PM accuracy was greater for the focal condition than for the nonfocal condition and was greater when PM importance was emphasized.

Ongoing task

Lexical decision accuracy was near ceiling (M = .97) (all Fs < 1). For response times, we excluded the first two trials, target trials, PM false alarms, and the two trials following targets and false alarms. Only word trials were included, and we excluded incorrect lexical decisions and response times that were greater than 3SDs from participants’ grand mean. There were main effects of focality, F(1, 188) = 13.12, p < .001, η 2p = .07, and importance, F(1, 188) = 8.02, p = .01, η 2p = .04, but no interaction, F < 1. Response times were greater for the nonfocal than for the focal condition and greater when PM importance was emphasized.

Planned comparisons

The summary of the literature by Smith et al. (2007) indicated that, if present, nonfocal and focal PM costs will be observed as large and medium size effects, respectively. The power to detect the large and medium size effects was .99 and .80, respectively (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). There was no a priori reason to expect a certain effect size for recognition, so we report the power to detect a medium size effect (.80). The parameter d′ was used as the measure of discrimination. Old words were excluded from the recognition tasks analyses if they were incorrectly responded during the lexical decision task.

There was no cost for the focal-standard condition, as compared with the control condition as measured by lexical decision response time, t(142) = 1.47, p = .14, d = .26. In addition, participants in the focal-standard condition (M = .97) made more accurate lexical decisions than did the controls (M = .96), t(142) = 2.12, p = .04, d = .39. On the recognition task, there were no differences in hit rate, t(142) = 1.01, p = .31, d = .18, false alarm, t < 1, or discrimination, t(142) = 1.37, p = .17, d = .23, between the focal-standard condition and controls.

Participants in the focal-important condition made slower lexical decisions than did the controls, t(142) = 4.31, p < .001, d = .72. There was, however, a trend for participants in the focal-important condition (M = .97) to make more accurate lexical decisions than did controls (M = .96), t(142) = 1.98, p = .05, d = .37. There was no correlation between lexical decision response time and PM accuracy, r = .003, p = .98, and no significant difference in response time to the four word trials preceding PM hits (M = 639 ms), as compared with the four word trials preceding PM misses (M = 611 ms), t(16) = 1.37, p = .19, d = .17, indicating that the cost was not functional to PM. There were no differences in hit rate, t(142) = 1.27, p = .21, d = .21, false alarm, t < 1, or discrimination, t < 1, between the focal-important condition and controls.

Participants in the nonfocal-standard condition made slower lexical decisions than did controls, t(142) = 5.06, p < .001, d = .84, with no difference in accuracy, t(142) = 1.62, p = .11, d = .29. Lexical decision response time was positively correlated with PM accuracy, r = .37, p = .01, and response times to the four word trials preceding PM hits (M = 692 ms) were slower than those to the four word trials preceding PM misses (M = 650 ms), t(39) = 2.57, p = .01, d = .39, both of which indicate that the cost was functional to PM. Participants in the nonfocal-standard condition had a higher hit rate, t(142) = 5.03, p < .001, d = .90, and higher discrimination, t(142) = 3.36, p < .001, d = .57, than did controls, with no difference in false alarm, t < 1. For the nonfocal-standard condition, discrimination was positively correlated with PM accuracy, r = .31, p = .03. There was no difference in lexical decision response time for those words subsequently recognized (M = 698 ms), as compared with those not recognized (M = 688 ms), t(47) = 1.68, p = .32, d = .08.

Participants in the nonfocal-important condition made slower lexical decisions than did controls, t(142) = 7.19, p < .001, d = 1.2, with no difference in accuracy, t(142) = 1.70, p = .09, d = .31. Lexical decision response time was positively correlated with PM accuracy, r = .33, p = .02, and response time to the four word trials preceding PM hits (M = 737 ms) were slower, as compared with the four word trials preceding PM misses (M = 662 ms), t(37) = 5.55, p < .001, d = .54, both of which indicate that the cost was functional to PM. Participants in the nonfocal-important condition had a higher hit rate, t(142) = 3.51, p < .001, d = .62, and had higher discrimination, t(142) = 2.62, p = .01, d = .48, than did controls, with no difference in false alarms, t < 1. For the nonfocal-important condition, discrimination and PM accuracy were not correlated, r = .12, p = .42. There was no difference in lexical decision response time for those words that were subsequently recognized (M = 737 ms), as compared with those not recognized (M = 728 ms), t(47) = 1.04, p = .31, d = .06.

Discussion

We have introduced a new dependent measure for examining whether participants differentially process individual nontarget items when they have PM requirements. Participants completed a lexical decision task with embedded PM requirements, followed by a surprise recognition test for nontarget ongoing task words. PM performance in both focal conditions was high. There were costs for the focal-important condition, but this cost was not functional to PM. It is likely that participants relied on spontaneous retrieval under focal conditions, without the need to maintain PM cognitive control processes on nontarget trials (Harrison & Einstein, 2010). The cognitive control processes underlying costs in response to the PM importance manipulation would not have involved accessing semantic relationships and, thus, would not have benefited recognition. Costs to ongoing tasks were observed for both nonfocal conditions, and these costs were functional to PM. Both nonfocal conditions had an increased hit rate and increased discrimination on the recognition task, relative to controls. Furthermore, for the nonfocal standard condition, there was a positive correlation between discrimination and PM accuracy, suggesting that the improved recognition reflected processes employed during the ongoing task that were beneficial to PM.

We interpret these data as evidence that participants in the nonfocal conditions engaged specific PM processes to evaluate individual ongoing task items for their PM status. Specifically, participants in the nonfocal condition mapped the semantic features of letter strings onto the semantic features of their PM category. This elaboration in processing increased the incidental learning of nontargets and enhanced their subsequent recognition (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). Our interpretation is in line with several PM theories that claim that individuals “check” or “monitor” for PM targets (Einstein & McDaniel, 2005; Guynn, 2003; Savine et al., 2012; Smith, 2010). We are not claiming that generic PM processes play no role in PM retrieval. However, processes such as preparing to switch between ongoing task and PM task responses (Smith, 2010), or the instantiation of retrieval modes (Guynn, 2003), should not influence the incidental learning of individual nontargets or their subsequent recognition.

The costs for the focal-importance and the nonfocal standard conditions were of similar effect size. Thus, the lack of recognition benefit for the focal-importance condition indicates that it was the type of item-level processing required to detect nonfocal targets, rather than the prolonged study time, that underlies the nonfocal recognition benefit. In line with this, for the nonfocal conditions, lexical decision response times were not longer for words subsequently recognized, as compared with those not subsequently recognized. Costs to ongoing tasks potentially reflect both specific and generic PM processes. It is possible, then, that the semantic mapping of items to the PM category contributes to only a portion of the nonfocal PM costs, in which case we would not necessarily expect a strong within-subjects relationship between the magnitude of costs and hit rates on the recognition test. Future research is needed to further examine the extent to which the cost to ongoing task measure and the recognition benefit measure we have introduced in the present article reflect similar underlying cognitive control processes.

The present data may appear at odds with the finding of Cook, Marsh, Clark-Foos, and Meeks (2007) that PM tasks imposed on participants when lists of words were studied decreased the subsequent recall of those words. Cook et al. concluded that PM task requirements during the study phase drew attention away from the creation of interitem associations that support free recall. In contrast, our recognition advantage depended on the incidental learning that occurred during the ongoing task when items were differentially processed for the PM task.

In conclusion, we have demonstrated that examining participants’ recognition of nontarget words previously presented in ongoing tasks can be informative regarding understanding the processes underlying PM. Continuing to use different dependent measures should provide greater constraints for the various theoretical propositions presented in the event-based PM literature.