Agrillo, C., & Bisazza, A (2014). Spontaneous versus trained numerical abilities. A comparison between the two main tools to study numerical competence in non-human animals. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 234, 82-91.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G., & Bisazza, A. (2008). Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition, 11, 495-503.
Article
Google Scholar
Beran, M. J. (2007). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) enumerate sequentially presented sets of items using analog numerical representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 42-54.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bortot, M., Stancher, G., & Vallortigara, G. (2020). Transfer from number to size reveals abstract coding of magnitude in honeybees. iScience, 23, 101122.
Cantlon, J. F., & Brannon, E. M. (2006). Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychological Science, 17, 401-406.
Article
Google Scholar
Carazo, P., Font, E., Forteza-Behrendt, E., & Desfilis, E. (2009). Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor: Evidence of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate? Animal Cognition, 12, 463-470.
Carey, S. (2009). The Origin of Concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Carpenter, F. L., Hixon, M. A., Temeles, E. J., Russell, R. W., & Paton, D. (1993). Exploitative compensation by subordinate age-sex classes of migrant rufous hummingbirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 33, 305-312.
Google Scholar
Chai, P., & Millard, D. (1997). Flight and size constraints: hovering performance of large hummingbirds under maximal loading. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 200, 2757-2763.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dadda, M., Piffer, L., Agrillo, C., & Bisazza, A. (2009). Spontaneous number representation in mosquitofish. Cognition, 112, 343-348.
Article
Google Scholar
Ditz, H. M., & Nieder, A. (2016). Numerosity representations in crows obey the Weber-Fechner law. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 283, 20160083.
Article
Google Scholar
Feigenson, L., & Carey, S. (2005). On the limits of infants’ quantification of small object arrays. Cognition, 97, 295-313.
Article
Google Scholar
Garland, A., Low, J., & Burns, K. C. (2012). Large quantity discrimination by North Island robins (Petroica longipes). Animal Cognition, 15, 1129-1140.
Article
Google Scholar
Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2007). Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): The effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 241-249.
Article
Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Carey, S., & Hauser, L. B. (2000). Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 267, 829-833.
Hauser, M. D., Tsao, F., Garcia, P., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton-top tamarins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, 1441-1446.
Article
Google Scholar
Healy, S. D., & Hurly, T. A. (1995). Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus): a field test. Animal Learning and Behavior, 23, 63-68.
Healy, S. D., & Hurly, T. A. (2013). What hummingbirds can tell us about cognition in the wild. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 8, 13–28.
Article
Google Scholar
Henderson, J., Hurly, T. A., Bateson, M., & Healy, S. D. (2006). Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus. Current Biology, 16, 512-515.
Hornsby, M. A. W., Hurly, T. A., Hamilton, C. E., Pritchard, D. J., & Healy, S. D. (2014). Wild, free-living rufous hummingbirds do not use geometric cues in a spatial task. Behavioural Processes, 108, 138-141.
Article
Google Scholar
Howard, S.R., Avarguès-Weber, A., Garcia, J.E., Greentree, A.D., & Dyer, A.G. (2018). Numerical ordering of zero in honey bees. Science, 360, 1124–1126.
Howard, S. R., Avarguès-Weber, A., Garcia, J.E., Greentree, A.D., & Dyer, A.G. (2019). Surpassing the subitizing threshold: Appetitive-aversive conditioning improves discrimination of numerosities in honeybees. Journal of Experimental Biology, 222, jeb.205658.
Article
Google Scholar
Howard, S. R., Schramme, J., Garcia, J.E., Ng, L., Avarguès-Weber, A., Greentree, A.D., & Dyer, A.G. (2020). Spontaneous quantity discrimination of artificial flowers by foraging honeybees. Journal of Experimental Biology, 223, jeb.223610.
Hurly, T. A. (2003). The twin threshold model: risk-intermediate foraging by rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus. Animal Behaviour, 66, 751-761.
Article
Google Scholar
Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2002). Cue learning by rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 209-223.
Hurly, T. A., Franz, S., & Healy, S. D. (2010). Do rufus hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus) use visual beacons? Animal Cognition, 13, 377-383.
Hyde, D. C. (2011). Two systems of non-symbolic numerical cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 150.
Article
Google Scholar
Jakobsen, H. B., & Kritjánsson, K. (1994). Influence of temperature and floret age on nectar secretion in Trifolium repens L. Annals of Botany, 74, 327-334.
Article
Google Scholar
López-Calleja, M. V., Fernàndez, M. J., & Bozinovic, F. (2003). The integration of energy and nitrogen balance in the hummingbird Sephanoides sephanoides. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206, 3349-3359.
Lucon-Xiccato, T., Petrazzini, M. E. M., Agrillo, C., & Bisazza, A. (2015). Guppies discriminate between two quantities of food items but prioritize item size over total amount. Animal Behaviour, 107, 183-191.
Marshall, R. E. S., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2012). Do a flower’s features help hummingbirds to learn its contents and refill rate? Animal Behaviour, 83, 1163-1169.
Article
Google Scholar
Marshall, R. E. S., Hurly, T. A., Sturgeon, J., Shuker, D. M., & Healy, S. D. (2013). What, where and when: deconstructing memory. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280, 20132184.
Google Scholar
Morgan, K. V., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2014). Individual differences in decision making by foraging hummingbirds. Behavioural Processes, 109, 195-200.
Article
Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987). Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African grey parrot: labelling of cardinal sets. Ethology, 75, 37-61.
Article
Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1994). Evidence for numerical competence in an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 36-44.
Article
Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2017). Avian numerical cognition: a review and brief comparisons to non-avian species. In C. ten Cate & S. D. Healy (Eds.), Avian Cognition (pp. 184-207). Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., & Carey, S. (2012). Grey parrot number acquisition: the inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list. Cognition, 125, 219-232.
Article
Google Scholar
Pleasants, J. M. (1983). Nectar production patterns in Ipompsis aggregate (Polemoniaceae). American Journal of Botany, 70, 1468-1475.
Article
Google Scholar
Rugani, R., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2008). Discrimination of small numerosities in young chicks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 34, 388-399.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, L. (2013). Numerical abstraction in young domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Discrimination of large numbers. Plos One, 8, e65265.
Samuels, M., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2014). Colour cues facilitate learning flower refill schedules in wild hummingbirds. Behavioural Processes, 109, 157-163.
Article
Google Scholar
Tello-Ramos, M. C., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2014). Female hummingbirds do not relocate rewards using colour cues. Animal Behaviour, 93, 129-133.
Article
Google Scholar
Tello-Ramos, M. C., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2015a). Traplining in hummingbirds: flying short-distance sequences amongst several locations. Behavioral Ecology, 26, 812-819.
Tello-Ramos, M. C., Hurly, T. A., Higgott, C., & Healy, S. D. (2015b). Time-place learning in wild, free-living hummingbirds. Animal Behaviour, 104, 123-129.
Article
Google Scholar
Tello-Ramos, M. C., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2019). From a sequential pattern, temporal adjustments emerge in hummingbird traplining. Integrative Zoology, 14, 182-192.
Article
Google Scholar
Uller, C., Jaeger, R., Guidry, G., & Martin, C. (2003). Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian. Animal Cognition, 6, 105-112.
Article
Google Scholar
Vámos, T. I. F., Tello-Ramos, M. C., Hurly, T. A., & Healy, S. D. (2020). Numerical ordinality in a wild nectarivore. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 287, 20201269.