There is a growing body of evidence that emotional information may spontaneously capture attention in various tasks, including visual search (Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001), spatial orienting (Brosch, Sander, Pourtois, & Scherer, 2008), or attentional blink (Anderson & Phelps, 2001), and across various stimulus categories, such as faces, words, or images. Such effects are generally taken to indicate privileged access to awareness for emotionally significant stimuli relative to neutral stimuli when attentional resources are limited in space or time. One approach to study the degree to which emotion captures attention is to ask participants to perform a cognitive task in the context of task-irrelevant, emotional pictures. If the emotional pictures attract attention, as observed, for instance, in a performance decrement in the cognitive task, one could infer that the emotional content is processed even when attention is directed toward another task and location.

Some previous studies that examined the extent to which irrelevant emotional material is processed have employed event-related potentials (ERPs) to assess neural activity associated with emotional processing by specifically focusing on a late-onset positivity component that starts around 300 ms after stimulus onset at central and central-parietal sites. This ERP component, which also has been referred to as the Late Positive Potential (LPP) (Ito, Larsen, Smith, & Cacioppo, 1998; Schupp et al., 2000; Weinberg & Hajcak, 2011), is taken as a neural index of sustained attention to emotional material (Schupp et al., 2000; Schupp, Flaisch, Stockburger, & Junghöfer, 2006) and is thought to reflect conscious representation of motivationally salient stimuli up to the level of semantic meaning (Hajcak, Dunning, & Foti, 2009; Schupp et al., 2006). Some studies show that it is larger for emotional compared with neutral content (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000; Foti, Hajcak, & Dien, 2009; Weinberg & Hajcak, 2010). There also is some evidence that the late-onset positivity is particularly sensitive to valence, more particularly a bias toward negative emotional content in younger adults (Huang & Luo, 2006; Ito et al., 1998) and a bias toward positive emotional content in older adults (Kisley, Wood, & Burrows, 2007; Mathieu et al., 2014).

Several ERP studies with younger adults have examined the degree to which emotion can capture attention by requiring participants to perform a cognitive task in the context of task-irrelevant, emotional pictures. Empirical results from these studies have suggested that ERPs to emotional versus neutral pictures are eliminated when pictures are spatially unattended (De Cesarei, Codispoti, & Schupp, 2009; Eimer, Holmes, & McGlone, 2003; Holmes, Kiss, & Eimer, 2006; Holmes, Vuilleumier, & Eimer, 2003; MacNamara & Hajcak, 2009). For example, when each trial showed a vertical pair of faces and a horizontal pair of houses (or vice versa), the late-onset positivity to fearful faces was eliminated when the faces were unattended (Holmes et al., 2003). Also, the late-onset positivity to emotional faces (Eimer et al., 2003) was eliminated when the faces were shown in the periphery and participants performed a cognitive task at fixation. Likewise, the late-onset positivity to emotional images was eliminated if these pictures were shown in the periphery and were unattended (De Cesarei et al., 2009; MacNamara & Hajcak, 2009). The late-onset positivity was even eliminated when faces were shown at fixation and participants performed a simple one-back task on line bars presented to the left and right of the faces (Holmes et al., 2006). The simplest explanation for these findings can be found in perceptual load theory (Lavie, 1995), which argues that when perceptual load is larger, subjects prioritize task-relevant stimuli, resulting in effective rejection of task-irrelevant distractors. Thus, these findings indicate that ERPs to emotional material might be eliminated when attentional resources are sufficiently depleted by the main task.

Although findings from the ERP studies with younger adults suggest that perception of the background emotional stimuli depends on the task demands associated with the foreground task, it is still an open question whether age-related differences exist in how task demands modulate ERPs for emotional material. Given that we are particularly interested in age-related differences in selective attention for emotional material, it is important to understand how aging is associated with cognitive processes involved in selective attention. The two basic aspects of processing involved in selective attention are inhibition and activation. Inhibition reflects the suppression of the processing of task-irrelevant information, preventing it from gaining access to working memory; activation reflects the enhancement of processing of task-relevant information. According to the inhibitory deficit hypothesis of aging, activation-related processes are mostly preserved in old age, whereas inhibitory functioning declines (Hasher, Stoltzfus, Zacks, & Rypma, 1991; Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Lustig, Hasher, & Zacks, 2007). There is both behavioral and neural evidence for the position that older adults have difficulty in suppressing distractors (Gazzaley et al., 2008; Gazzaley, Cooney, McEvoy, Knight, & D'esposito, 2005). An important caveat in these studies, however, is that the age-related suppression deficiency in the neural data often disappear once older adults’ working memory performance is matched with that of younger adults. Similarly, in her review of the literature, Fabiani (2012) notes that many of the neuroscientific findings about age differences in neural activity associated with inhibition functioning disappear when task difficulty is equated across groups, suggesting that the observed age-related differences in neural data and behavioral performance may not result from the aging process per se but from group differences in mean difficulty levels (see also Verhaeghen, 2011; Verhaeghen, Geigerman, Yang, Montoya, & Rahnev, 2019).

Thus, in the current study, we investigated ERP correlates of age-related differences in processing of task-relevant and -irrelevant emotional material by employing a selective attention task that required younger and older adults to selectively attend to either centrally presented pictures (emotional or nonemotional) or to flanking line bars, concurrently presented on the screen. Our main interest was in comparing age-related differences in the extent to which emotional pictures were still being processed (relative to baseline neutral pictures) at the neural level when they were distractors versus when they were the focus of the task. To circumvent the task difficulty confound mentioned above and equate performance between age groups on a neutral baseline version of the task, we calibrated task difficulty individually (using a similar procedure outlined in Verhaeghen et al., 2019), so that all subjects performed at identical levels in the nonemotional bar task with neutral pictures. The calibration procedure enabled us to examine ERP correlates of age-related differences in inhibiting task-irrelevant emotional material over and beyond task-irrelevant neutral material in the bar task.

The task design additionally allows us to test two competing theories of emotion and aging. According to socioemotional selectivity theory (SST) (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999), motivational priorities change as a function of time left in life. When individuals perceive the future as expansive, they are more likely to focus on information-seeking goals, such as acquiring new information and meeting new people. When individuals perceive the time left as limited, they are more likely to focus on emotional-regulation goals. Given that older adults are more likely to perceive their future as limited, their information processing is claimed to shift toward positive information in order to increase their well-being. Consistent with this theory, many studies show that older adults attend to and remember positive information better than negative information (for a meta-analysis, see Reed, Chan, & Mikels, 2014), whereas younger adults focus on negative information more than positive information (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001). In contrast to SST, the aging and brain model (ABM) argues that age-related decline in the brain selectively diminishes emotional arousal in response to negative stimuli (but not positive stimuli), and, as a result, older adults fail to process negative stimuli and thus experience less negative affect (Cacioppo, Berntson, Bechara, Tranel, & Hawkley, 2011). This perspective then claims that the positivity effect in older adults results from decreased focus on negative material rather than an increased focus on positive material. These two views make opposing arguments about the source of the positivity effect in older age, and so our data, which examine patterns in emotional modulation as a function of task relevancy via ERPs, could be helpful in determining the source of the age-related differences in emotional processing—either an upregulating of positive content, or a downregulating of negative content.

Expected results

Picture task

In the picture task, emotional modulation would change based on ERP components and age. The literature suggests that the late-onset positivity component would be sensitive to motivationally salient stimuli (Carretié et al., 2001; Ito et al., 1998; Kisley et al., 2007). Thus, we expect that this component would capture both the positivity bias in older adults and the negativity bias in younger adults.

Regarding the source of the positivity bias in older adults, if the age-related positivity bias results from an increased focus on positive material as suggested by SST (Carstensen et al., 1999), then older adults would show larger ERPs for positive pictures compared with neutral and negative images relative to younger adults. If, however, the positivity bias is driven by a reduced negativity bias in older adults as suggested by ABM (Cacioppo et al., 2011), then older adults would show reduced ERPs for negative images compared with positive and neutral images compared with younger adults, with no age-related differences in the ERP amplitudes between positive and neutral images.

Bar task

In the bar task, the goal was to investigate age-related differences in emotional modulation of ERPs when pictures are unattended. Under the hypothesis that the often-reported, age-related deficit in inhibiting task-irrelevant emotional material is an artifact of differences between age groups at baseline, we would expect no age-related effects in ERPs for task-irrelevant material at all, given that the calibration procedure sets the baseline performance levels to be identical across age groups. Thus, if there is no age-related deficit in inhibiting task-irrelevant emotional, ERPs for task-irrelevant emotional and neutral material would be similar within and between age groups. If, however, there is an age-related deficit in inhibiting task-irrelevant emotional material, the bar task should yield age-related differences in the emotional ERP components even after performance is equated, because the task demand associated with the bar task may prevent older adults from exerting suppression of task-irrelevant emotional material. That is, older adults would show larger ERPs for task-irrelevant emotional material relative to neutral material during the bar task, whereas ERPs for emotional and neutral pictures would be similar for younger adults.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-four younger and 35 older adults participated in the experiment. All participants provided written, informed consent before participating in this study according to a protocol approved by the Georgia Institute of Technology's Human Subjects Institutional Review Board and in accordance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. Older participants were recruited from the community; they received cash payment ($10/hour) as compensation for participation.Footnote 1 Younger adults were students at the Georgia Institute of Technology and participated in the study in return for course credit. Data from two younger and three older participants were excluded from further analysis due to slow response times (response times 2 standard deviations above the group mean) and/or more than 50% trials with ERP artifacts (e.g., alpha, eye movements, blinks, muscle tension). The mean age of the remaining 32 younger adults (16 females) was 19.97 (SD = 1.58); mean age of the remaining 32 older adults (18 females) was 70.59 (SD = 4.1). All participants were right-handed, with normal or corrected to normal vision, with no reports of psychiatric or neurological disorders, vascular disease, or color blindness. None of the participants were taking CNS-active medications. None of the participants reported that they were suffering from depression or anxiety. Sixty-six percent of the older adults reported being White/Caucasian and 34% Black/African-American; 44% of the young adults reported being White/Caucasian, 9% of Hispanic descent, 9% reported being of Black/African-American descent, 32% reported being of Asian descent, and 6% reported being of other descent. Older adults had completed more years of education (M = 16.97, SD = 2.96) than younger adults (M = 13.83, SD = 1.6) [t(62) = 5.29, p < 0.001]. Younger adults (M = 60.69, SD = 11.5) performed significantly better than older adults (M = 46.66, SD = 7.16) on a symbol-digit test (Smith, 1973) [t(62) = 5.86, p < 0.001]. Older adults’ performance (M = 36.28, SD = 2.8) on the Shipley Vocabulary test (Shipley, 1946) was significantly higher than performance of younger adults (M = 31.06, SD = 3.17) [t(62) = 6.98, p < 0.001].

Stimuli

Given that older adults, relative to younger adults, tend to rate emotional pictures as more extreme for both valence and arousal (Grühn & Scheibe, 2008), it is important to select stimuli for which subjective ratings are matched between age groups. To this end, we conducted a picture rating study prior to the experiment proper. In this rating experiment, independent groups of younger and older adults rated valence and arousal level of emotional stimuli. For the picture rating experiment, a total of 720 images, divided evenly between valence categories (neutral, negative, positive) as judged by the experimenters, were selected from the International Affective Picture Set (IAPS) (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1999), the Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS) (Marchewka, Żurawski, Jednoróg, & Grabowska, 2014), the Open Affective Standardized Image Set (OASIS) (Kurdi, Lozano, & Banaji, 2017), the Geneva affective picture database (GAPED) (Dan-Glauser & Scherer, 2011), and Google (using search terms, such as celebration, mutilation, etc.). Because previous research showed that there are age-related differences in ratings of pictures with radical/exciting sports and erotic content (Backs, da Silva, & Han, 2005), pictures from these semantic categories were excluded. Additionally, an effort was made to select images with similar visual complexity (indexed by jpeg size; Donderi, 2006; Marchewka et al., 2014), because a previous study by Wiens, Sand, and Olofsson (2011b) showed that picture composition (i.e., figure vs. scene) confounds ERP amplitudes.

Participants were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) and completed the study online. Participants rated each presented picture in terms of valence (on a 1-9 point scale, where 1 was extremely pleasant and 9 was extremely unpleasant) and arousal (on a 1-9 point scale, where 1 was extremely aroused and 9 was extremely calm). Procedural details can be found in the Appendix. We selected a total of 462 pictures that had been rated as positive and arousing (N = 140; valence less than 4.25 and arousal less than 6.4), negative and arousing (N = 140; valence greater than 5.75, arousal less than 6), and neutral and non-arousing (N = 182; valence between 4.5 and 5.5, arousal greater than 6). Of the 182 neutral pictures, 42 were used in the calibration experiment that participants performed prior to the experiment proper. Complexity of pictures selected for the experiment (indexed by JPEG size) did not differ significantly between valence categories. The mean valence and arousal ratings for each picture category and age group for the stimuli included in the study are presented in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1. Mean valence, minimum, and maximum ratings for the stimuli
Table 2. Mean arousal, minimum, and maximum ratings for stimuli

Procedure

Before the session, each participant signed a consent form, filled out the Shipley Vocabulary test and symbol-digit test measuring verbal ability and processing speed, respectively. Next, they completed the calibration experiment which was followed by the actual experiment. EEG was only recorded during the actual experiment. In both experiments, the subjects sat approximately 57 cm from the display. Both experiments were run on a 17-inch CRT monitor (1024 x 768 pixel resolution; 75 Hz refresh rate) using MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA) and the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997).

Calibration experiment

Trial structure for the calibration experiment is presented in Fig. 1. Each trial started with the presentation of a fixation cross for 1,500 ms. Next, a centrally presented neutral picture (6.5° x 5.5°) and two bars (0.3° x 2.75°) positioned peripherally (2° to the right and left of the central picture) were presented briefly. Presentation duration of the stimuli was 200 ms for the younger participants and 250 ms for the older participants, below the time needed to execute a saccade. Presentation duration for older adults was increased to allow for extra processing time due to age-related slowing; the slowing factor was derived from the typical age-related slowing factor for saccadic eye movements (Verhaeghen, 2013). Only neutral pictures were used for the calibration experiment (see Padmala & Pessoa, 2014, for a similar calibration procedure). Following the stimulus presentation, the subjects were asked to withhold their response for 800 ms, during which period a blank screen was presented (this mimics the procedure in the actual experiment). Next, the subjects were shown a question mark on the center of the screen and asked to indicate whether the orientations of the peripheral bars matched.

Fig. 1.
figure 1

Trial structure for the calibration experiment: Each trial started with presentation of a fixation cross for 1,500 ms. After that, a neutral picture (represented by a blue square) flanked by two bars was shown briefly. This was followed by presentation of a blank screen for 800 ms during which the subjects were asked to withholder their responses. Immediately after, the subjects were shown a question mark and decided as quickly and accurately as possible whether or not the orientations of the bars matched.

We used the QUEST algorithm (Watson & Pelli, 1983), an adaptive staircase procedure, to determine the 75% discrimination threshold for the angular difference of the bars for each subject. The number of trials needed to estimate the 75% threshold was determined by the algorithm and the experiment stopped when the performance criterion was achieved for each participant.Footnote 2 The calibration experiment took approximately 10 min to complete.

Actual experiment

Following the calibration experiment, EEG capping was done for the actual experiment. The trial structure in the actual experiment (Fig. 2) was the same as the calibration experiment except that each trial started with the presentation of a small circle for 1,500 ms to allow the subjects to blink (if necessary) and thus to avoid eye blink artifacts during stimulus presentation. The individual threshold obtained from the calibration experiment was used to set differences in angle of bar rotation on an individual basis. There were two tasks: In the bar task, subjects were instructed to ignore the central images and to indicate whether the orientations of the peripheral bars matched; they did this while maintaining fixation on the center of the screen, where the picture appeared. In the picture task, subjects were instructed to ignore the bars and to indicate whether the picture was presented in black and white.Footnote 3 Likewise, they maintained fixation at the center of the screen, where the picture appeared.

Fig. 2.
figure 2

Trial structure for the actual experiment: Each trial started with presentation of a circle for 1,500 ms to signal to the subjects to blink. Then, a fixation cross was shown for 1,500 ms. After that, a neutral, negative, or positive picture (represented here by a blue square) flanked by two bars was shown briefly. This was followed by presentation of a blank screen for 800 ms during which the subjects were asked to withholder their responses. Immediately after, the subjects were shown a question mark and decided as quickly and accurately as possible as whether or not the orientations of the bars matched (bar task) or whether the picture was presented in black and white (picture task)

Subjects performed the bar and picture tasks in separate blocks; task order was counterbalanced across participants. The picture and bar task in the current study were adopted from previous studies employing similar tasks (see Sand & Wiens, 2011; Wiens, Sand, Norberg, & Andersson, 2011a, for the picture task; see Erthal et al., 2005; Padmala & Pessoa, 2014; Pessoa, Kastner, & Ungerleider, 2002; Verhaeghen et al., 2019, for the bar task). Each task consisted of 5 blocks of 42 trials; each block contained an equal number of neutral, positive, and negative images, an equal number of color and black and white images, and an equal number of match and mismatch stimuli. The order of neutral and emotional images, color and black and white images, and match and mismatch bars within a block was randomized. Additionally, picture color was counterbalanced across participants, such that pictures that were presented in black and white for one half of the participants were presented in color for the other half, and vice versa. For mismatch bar stimuli, the angular difference between the two bars was created by rotating one of the bars either clockwise or counter clockwise one threshold value away from the original stimulus. The subjects were given feedback about accuracy at the end of each block. The whole session (calibration and experiment proper) lasted approximately 100 min.

EEG Acquisition

Scalp-recorded EEG data was collected from 32 Ag-Ag/Cl electrodes using an ActiveTwo amplifier system (Biosemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands). Electrodes were positioned according to the extended 10–20 system (Nuwer et al., 1998). Electrodes were located at left/right hemisphere locations (FP1/FP2, AF3/AF4, F3/F4, F7/F8, FC1/FC2, FC5/FC6, C3/C4, T7/T8, CP1/CP2, CP5/CP6, P3/P4, P7/P8, PO3/PO4, O1/O2) as well as midline sites (Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz). Two additional electrodes placed on the left and right mastoid processes were used as off-line references. Four additional electrodes were placed above and below the left eye to record a vertical electrooculogram (VEOG) and on the outer canthi of the left and right eyes to record a horizontal electrooculogram (HEOG). EEG was recorded with 24-bit resolution and a sampling rate of 512 Hz. All data processing, including filtering and extracting epochs, was performed in MATLAB using the ERPLAB (Lopez-Calderon & Luck, 2014) toolbox, except for artifact correction and rejection procedures which were performed using the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme & Makeig, 2004). For data processing, electrodes placed on the mastoids were used as off-line references and data were digitally band-pass filtered using a 2nd order infinite impulse response (IIR) Butterworth filter (half-amplitude cutoffs at 0.01 and 100 Hz, 12 dB/octave roll-off). Then, EEG segments were created from 200-ms prestimulus onset until 1,000-ms poststimulus onset. Artifacts were removed in 3 steps. First, manual artifact rejection procedure was applied to remove epochs containing non-ocular artifacts (e.g., large drift, electrode spikes, saturation). Second, independent component analysis was used to remove ocular artifacts components from the remaining epochs (Delorme & Makeig, 2004). Lastly, epochs containing uncorrected artifacts (±150 mV) were removed. Depending on the condition and age group, between 7.6% and 14% of trials were removed (median = 9.4%). Epochs were averaged separately for each participant, valence, electrode, and task. The averaged waveforms were digitally smoothed with a low-pass filter of 30 Hz. Only correct trials (i.e., hits and correct rejections) were included in the ERP analysis.

ERP Analysis

Based on the extant ERP studies reporting late-onset positivity in similar experimental designs (Holmes et al., 2006; Sand & Wiens, 2011), the late-onset positivity was examined at central and centro-parietal electrode sites (C3, C4, CP1, CP2).

To detect time windows where younger and older adults showed reliable emotional processing in the picture and bar tasks, ERPs elicited by the emotional material versus neutral images at electrode sites of interest in each task and within each age group were submitted to two-tailed cluster-based permutation tests with a family-wise alpha level of 0.05 (Bullmore et al., 1999) using the Mass Univariate ERP Toolbox (Groppe, Urbach, & Kutas, 2011). These cluster-based multiple comparisons corrections provide control over the weak family-wise error rate while maintaining high sensitivity to detect broadly distributed ERP effects (Groppe et al., 2011; Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). The ERP data were down-sampled to 128 Hz before these mass univariate analyses. All time points (measured every 4 ms; 90 total time points) between 100 and 800 ms at electrode sites of interest (C3, C4, CP1, CP2) were included in the analyses (i.e., 360 total comparisons). To estimate the distribution of the null hypothesis, 2,500 random within-participant permutations of the data were applied, which is more than twice the number recommended for a family-wise alpha level of 0.05 (Manly, 1997). For each permutation, all t-scores corresponding to uncorrected p-values of 0.05 or less were formed into clusters with any neighboring t-scores meeting the same criteria. Electrodes within 5 cm of one another were considered spatial neighbors and adjacent time points were considered temporal neighbors. The cluster “mass” was defined as the sum of the t-scores in each cluster, and the most extreme cluster mass in each of the 2,501 sets of tests was recorded and used to estimate the distribution of the null hypothesis. The permutation cluster mass percentile ranking of each cluster from the observed data was used to derive its p-value. The p-value of the cluster was assigned to each member of the cluster and t-scores that were not included in a cluster were assigned a p-value of 1. Once time windows of interest for ERPs reflecting emotion processing were determined via the cluster-based permutation analyses for each age group within the each task, a mixed ANOVA including factors of Task, Valence, and Group was performed for further statistical testing of ERP effects across tasks and age groups.Footnote 4

Results

Calibration Experiment Results

Bar task performance of younger adults (M = 0.75, SD = 0.04) in the calibration experiment was similar to that of the older adults (M = 0.76, SD = 0.06) [t(62) = 0.33, p = 0.75], also indicating that the desired level of accuracy (75%) was indeed achieved. Younger adults (M = 10.34°, SD = 4.11°) had lower thresholds for angular disparity than older adults (M = 15.3°, SD = 6.79°), [t(62) = 3.53, p = 0.001].

Actual Experiment: Behavioral Results

A Group (Younger, Older) x Task (Bar, Picture) x Valence (Positive, Negative, Neutral) ANOVA was conducted on correct responses and corresponding reaction times. Proportions of correct and corresponding response times for negative, positive, and neutral valences in each task are presented in Table 3. With regard to accuracy, the ANOVA revealed a main effect of Task [F(1,62) = 294.11, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.83] and a significant Task x Valence interaction [F(2,124) = 5.10, p = 0.007, η2p = 0.08]. The Task x Valence interaction reflects that accuracy for both negative and positive images was larger than accuracy for neutral images in the picture task [all ts > 1.91, ps < 0.001], while there was no effect of valence on accuracy in the bar task [all ts < 0.64, ps > 0.5].Footnote 5 Additionally, accuracy was higher in the picture task than in the bar task regardless of image valence [all ts > 13.76, ps < 0.001]. Note that the picture task clearly shows a ceiling effect, potentially masking true emotion effects. Neither a main effect of valence nor interaction effects involving age group was significant (all Fs < 2.6, ps > 0.1). With regard to response times, the ANOVA revealed a main effect of Group [F(1,62) = 6.71, p = 0.01, η2p = 0.9], indicating that older adults responded slower than younger adults. Neither main effects of Task and Valence nor other interaction effects involving Age Group, Task, and/or Valence factors was significant [all Fs < 0.7, ps > 0.1].Footnote 6

Table 3. Proportion correct and corresponding reactions times in the picture and bar task for younger and older adults

Actual Experiment: ERP Results

The cluster-based permutation test

Raster diagrams visualizing the results of cluster-based permutation analyses for a contrast comparing ERPs to emotional and neutral images, and the corresponding topographic maps within each age group are presented in Fig. 3 for the picture task, and in Fig. 4 for the bar task. In the picture task, one significant positive cluster (p < 0.05) lasting from 564 to 775 ms was identified in the younger group and one significant positive cluster (p < 0.05) lasting from 463 to 650 ms was identified in the older group. In the bar task, one significant negative cluster (p < 0.05) lasting from 252 to 627 ms was identified in the younger group, whereas no significant positive or negative clusters (p > 0.24) were identified in the older group. Thus, based on the cluster-based permutation analyses, ERP effects were defined as the mean amplitude from 463 to 650 ms time window in both tasks for the older group. For the younger group, ERP effects were defined as the mean amplitude from 564 to 775 ms time window in the picture task and from 252 to 627 ms time window in the bar task.

Fig. 3.
figure 3

Picture task results. Left-hand panel: Raster diagrams visualizing the results of cluster-based permutation analyses on ERPs for the contrast comparing emotional and neutral images. Each cell represents the result of a t-test on the contrast. The y-axis indicates electrodes. The x-axis indicates time in 13-ms increments. Color indicates that the test was significant (p < 0.05) after correcting for multiple comparisons. Right-hand panel: Topographical maps reflecting difference scores for ERPs to emotional versus neutral images. Circles represent electrode locations (C3/C4, CP1/CP2) for the cluster analysis

Fig. 4.
figure 4

Bar task results. Left-hand panel: Raster diagrams visualizing the results of cluster-based permutation analyses on ERPs for the contrast comparing emotional and neutral images. Each cell represents the result of a t-test on the contrast. The y-axis indicates electrodes. The x-axis indicates time in 13 ms increments. Color indicates that the test was significant (p < 0.05) after correcting for multiple comparisons. Right-hand panel: Topographical maps reflecting difference scores for ERPs to emotional versus neutral images. Circles represent electrode locations (C3/C4, CP1/CP2) for the cluster analysis

ERP effects

A Task x Valence x Group ANOVA revealed a main effect of Group [F(1,62) = 28.24, p < .001, η2p = 0.32] that was modified by interactions with Task [F(1,62) = 37.58, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.38], and Valence [F(2,124) = 4.56, p = 0.01, η2p = 0.07]. An interaction between Valence and Task was also significant [F(2,124) = 30.57, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.33]. Importantly, there was a Task x Valence x Group interaction [F(2,124) = 5.49, p = 0.005, η2p = 0.08]. To follow-up this interaction, we conducted analyses within the picture and bar task separately in two steps. First, we examined the effect of Valence within-age group. In the second step, we examined between-age group differences on ERP difference scores reflecting positivity (positive minus neutral), negativity (negative minus neutral), and emotional salience (average emotional minus neutral) effects at the electrode sites of interest in the time windows associated with ERP effects in each age group.

Picture task

Figure 5 displays the average ERP waveforms elicited by negative, neutral, and positive images obtained from correct trials in the picture task for younger and older adults.

Fig. 5.
figure 5

ERPs elicited by negative, neutral, and positive images in the picture task by younger and older adults. Green arrows indicate the late-onset positivity effect in younger (564-775 ms) and older (463-650 ms) adults

Younger adults

The ANOVA during the 564-775 ms time window revealed a main effect of Valence [F(2,62) = 7.41, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.19]. As can be seen in Fig. 5, follow-up analyses (using a Bonferroni corrected α = 0.0167) showed that ERPs were larger for both positive (M = 6.43 μV, SD = 8.04) and negative (M = 5.8 μV, SD = 6.68) images than neutral (M = 3.15 μV, SD = 7.39) images in the picture task [all ts > 3.25, ps < 0.004] with no significant difference between negative and positive images [t(31) = 0.58, p = 0.56].

Older adults

The ANOVA during the 463-650 ms time window revealed a main effect of Valence [F(2,62) = 12.49, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.29]. As shown in Fig. 5, follow-up analyses (using a Bonferroni-corrected α = 0.0167) showed that ERPs were larger for positive (M = 10.71 μV, SD = 7.1) than negative (M = 8.74 μV, SD = 7.2) and neutral (M = 7.08 μV, SD = 6.59) images in the picture task [all ts > 2.8, ps < 0.001] with no difference between negative and neutral images [t(31) =2.34, p = 0.02].

Between-group analyses

Figure 6 displays difference waveforms reflecting positivity (positive minus neutral), negativity (negative minus neutral), and emotional salience (average emotional minus neutral) effects in the picture task. During the window associated with the late-onset positivity component in each age group, the analyses revealed that the magnitude of emotion related ERP effects was similar across age groups [all ts < 1, ps > 0.1].

Fig. 6.
figure 6

Difference waveforms for ERP difference scores reflecting positivity (top row), negativity (middle row), and emotional salience (bottom row) effects in the picture task at electrode sites of interest for younger (dotted black) and older (solid black) adults; no significant age-related differences were found

Bar task

Figure 7 displays the average ERP waveforms elicited by negative, neutral, and positive images obtained from correct trials in the bar task for younger and older adults.

Fig. 7.
figure 7

ERPs elicited by negative, neutral, and positive images in the bar task by younger and older adults. Orange arrows indicate the sustained negativity effect observed in younger adults (252-400 ms); green arrows indicate the late-onset positivity effect in younger (401-627 ms) and older (463-650 ms) adults

Younger adults

As shown in Fig. 7, visual inspection of ERP waveforms suggested that the late-onset positivity in younger adults was preceded by a non-hypothesized sustained negative deflection. Therefore, we conducted two ANOVAs to investigate these qualitatively different ERP effects separately. Regarding the negative deflection, the ANOVA during the 252-400 ms time window revealed a main effect of Valence [F(2,62) = 19.57, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.39]. Follow-up analyses (using a Bonferroni-corrected α = 0.0167) showed that ERPs were larger for negative (M = −8.17, SD = 9.68) and positive (M = −10.35 μV, SD = 9.89) images than neutral (M = −4.37 μV, SD = 11.63) images [all ts > 3.5, ps < 0.01] with no significant difference between negative and positive images [t(31) = 2.57, p = 0.02]. Regarding the late-onset positivity, the ANOVA during the 401-627 ms time window revealed a main effect of Valence [F(2,62) = 10.24, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.25]. Follow-up analyses (using a Bonferroni corrected α = 0.0167) showed that ERPs were larger for neutral (M = 7.02, SD = 10.54) than positive (M = 1.85 μV, SD = 8.47) and negative (M = 2.74 μV, SD = 9.77) images [all ts > 3, ps < 0.01] with no significant difference between negative and positive images [t(31) = 0.8, p = 0.43].

Additionally, accuracy in the bar task was positively correlated with difference scores denoting positivity (r = 0.58, p = 0.001), negativity (r = 0.36, p = 0.04), and emotional salience (r = 0.51, p = 0.003) during the time window associated with late-onset positivity, suggesting that suppression of emotional material in the bar task increases accuracy in the bar task.

Older adults

The ANOVA during the 463-650 ms time windowFootnote 7 did not revealed a main effect of Valence [F(2,62) = 0.27, p = 0.76, η2p = 0.01]. ERPs were similar across Valence categories (Fig. 7). Because there were no effects of valence, difference scores associated with positivity, negativity, or valence were not correlated with bar task accuracy.

Between-group analyses

Figure 8 displays difference waveforms reflecting positivity (positive minus neutral), negativity (negative minus neutral), and emotional salience (average emotional minus neutral) effects in the bar task. During the window associated with the late-onset positivity component in each age group, the analyses (using a Bonferroni-corrected α = 0.0167) revealed that ERP difference scores reflecting the positivity and emotional salience effects were reliably larger in the younger group compared to the older group [all ts > 3, ps < 0.004], with no significant age-related difference regarding the negativity effect [t(62) = 2.29, p = 0.03].

Fig. 8.
figure 8

Difference waveforms for ERP difference scores reflecting positivity (top row), negativity (middle row), and emotional salience (bottom row) effects in the bar task at electrode sites of interest in time windows associated with ERP effects for younger (dotted black) and older (solid black) adults

Discussion

In the current study, we examined ERP correlates of age-related differences in processing of task-relevant and -irrelevant emotional material when performance between age groups was equated at baseline. We employed a selective attention task in which younger and older adults were presented with a picture (emotionally neutral, positive, or negative) flanked by two bars; the task was to denote whether the picture was presented in color or whether the orientation of the bars matched. There were three major findings. First, at the behavioral level, when images were task-relevant, accuracy was higher for emotional relative to neutral material regardless of age; when images were task-irrelevant, there was no interference from emotional material. Second, when images were task-relevant, ERPs provided evidence for emotion-based processing in the younger group; older adults showed more differentiated valence-based processing, reflecting a positivity effect. Third, when images were task-irrelevant, ERP patterns differed by age. Older adults showed effective inhibition of the emotional distractors (i.e., they processed emotional material at the level of neutral material); younger adults engaged in suppression (i.e., we found evidence for reduced processing of task-irrelevant emotional images relative to baseline neutral images). These results and their implications are discussed in more detail below.

Behavioral results

Although both younger and older showed higher accuracy for emotional relative to neutral images in the picture task, there was no evidence of valence-based effects (either a positivity or negativity bias) in either age group. Performance was close to ceiling, however, and this is likely to have masked potential valence-based effects. Crucially, in the bar task, neither accuracy nor response times differed as a function of emotion, suggesting that neither age group experienced intrusion from emotional material at the behavioral level. This absence of interference from task-irrelevant emotional images in the bar task seems to be in line with the load theory of attention (Lavie, 1995), which predicts that difficult tasks (like our bar task, which was engineered to yield 75% accuracy) leave no additional resources for processing task-irrelevant material. As is typical, we observed age-related slowing, reflected in longer response times in the older compared to the younger group in both picture and bar tasks.

ERP Results

Emotional processing in the picture task

The picture task offers insights into the neural responses to emotional pictures that were not explicitly scrutinized for emotional content. In the younger group, the late-onset positivity, which was observed from 564 to 775 ms, was larger for emotional relative to neutral images, suggesting that in this time window younger adults are sensitive to the difference in arousal value between stimuli. In the older group, the late-onset positivity, which was observed from 463 to 650 ms, was associated with larger ERPs for positive compared with negative and neutral images, suggesting that older adults showed increased attention to motivationally salient positive material, which in turn may reflect semantic elaboration focusing on the positive content. This finding is consistent with previous ERP studies showing larger late-onset positivity for positive relative to negative images (Kisley et al., 2007; Mathieu et al., 2014). It also may imply that the older adults engaged in some form of emotion-regulation mechanism that accentuated the positive content of the pictures (Carstensen et al., 1999).

Interestingly, although older adults showed a valence-based effect, reflecting a positivity bias, no valence-based effect reflecting negativity bias was observed in younger adults. Instead, the younger group showed an overall emotion-based effect. Although this ERP pattern is consistent with previous studies with younger adults showing an increase in magnitude of the late-onset positivity for both pleasant and unpleasant compared with neutral stimuli (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Schupp et al., 2000), it is inconsistent with some previous studies providing evidence for a negativity bias during the time window associated with the late-onset positivity (Ito et al., 1998). One possible reason for this may be that the valence effect might be moderated by arousal. Wiens and Syrjänen (2013) showed that when emotional material is task-relevant and arousal level of the stimuli ranged from medium to high, ERPs reflecting the late-onset positivity were similar for pleasant and unpleasant images. In contrast, extreme stimulus arousal values produced valence-based effects on ERPs. Although our negative images were more arousing than our positive and neutral images, we did not include extremely arousing negative images, which in turn may have prevented the negativity bias to appear in the younger group.

One of the goals of the picture task was to elucidate the underlying mechanisms for the positivity effect in older adults. We indeed observed an emotional positivity effect during the time window associated with the late-onset positivity component in the older adult group. Between-age-group comparisons, however, yielded no age-related differences regarding ERPs for either negativity, positivity, or emotional salience during the late-onset positivity. Taken together, this set of results provides partial support for SST (Carstensen et al., 1999) and rules out ABM (Cacioppo et al., 2011). Specifically, older adults showed a positivity effect without an age-related decline in processing of negative material. Thus, this positivity effect appears not to be driven by an age-related neural deficit in processing negative material as suggested by ABM. However, given that the magnitude of the positivity effect is similar across age groups, SST cannot be fully supported, because it predicts a larger positivity effect in older adults relative to their younger counterparts.

Emotional processing in the bar task

In the bar task, participants engaged in a cognitive task—judging whether the orientation of two slanted bars matched—while ignoring pictures (neutral, negative, or positive) projected at the center of the screen. Before the experiment, task performance was calibrated using neutral pictures as the distractors. Thus, the findings from this task shed light on the extent to which older and younger adults are capable of specifically inhibiting the emotional content of the pictures.

Older adults showed near-identical ERPs for task-irrelevant emotional and neutral images during the time window associated with late-onset positivity (450-650 ms), indicating that they did not process task-irrelevant emotional material over and beyond task-irrelevant neutral material in the bar task (i.e., distraction from emotionally charged and neutral material was similar). Thus, in line with the idea that at least some inhibitory deficits might be an artifact of task difficulty, the data suggest that when performance was equated across subjects in an emotionally neutral version of the task, older adults were indeed able to ignore task-irrelevant emotional material. This suggests, more generally, that when the main task is of sufficient difficulty, older adults are able to prioritize task-relevant stimuli, resulting in effective rejection of task-irrelevant material, as predicted by cognitive load theory (Lavie, 1995).

Different from older adults, ERP data from the younger group showed a pattern that reflects differential processing of task-irrelevant material. Specifically, ERPs to task-irrelevant emotional material relative to neutral material was evident during the time window associated with the sustained negative deflection preceding the later positivity (252-400 ms), suggesting that younger adults did perceive and process pictures that were supposed to be ignored up to the level of extracting their emotional content. This interference from emotional material indicates that younger adults were not able or willing to inhibit emotional distractors over and above neutral distractors. During the later portion of processing (401-627 ms), however, younger adults showed larger ERPs for neutral relative to emotional images—the opposite of what would be expected for unimpeded perception of emotion and also the opposite of the pattern that was found in the picture task, where ERPs were larger for emotional than neutral images. These findings suggest that after the initial breakthrough of the emotional content of the to-be-ignored pictures earlier in the processing stream, younger adults exhibited an attentional shift that allowed them to differentiate between neutral and emotional material at the later stage of processing and suppress the emotional content. Importantly, between-age-group comparisons regarding the magnitude of ERPs reflecting emotional salience (i.e., ERPs to emotional versus neutral images) indicated larger suppression in the younger group compared with the older group.

The data thus reveal an interesting and unexpected age-related difference: While older adults showed similar ERPs for task-irrelevant images regardless of valence during the time window associated with the late-onset positivity component, the younger group showed evidence for suppression of task-irrelevant emotional images (as reflected by reduced ERPs for task-irrelevant emotional images relative baseline neutral images). This suppression is effective: We found significant correlations between the degree of emotional suppression and bar task accuracy. Thus, we find an interesting dissociation, possibly hinting at strategic differences between the two age groups: Older adults achieve their 75% level of accuracy by effectively ignoring (i.e., inhibiting) the task-irrelevant emotional stimuli (as instructed); younger adults achieve this same criterion by additional processing that suppresses the emotional content of the to-be-ignored images. It is not clear if this strategic difference is an age-related compensation strategy on the part of the older adults (where the task becomes “easier to perform” if stimuli are simply ignored) or if our result is an artifact of the specific level of difficulty chosen (which might still be easily attainable by younger adults by using active suppression). This raises questions about the level of effort required in the bar task across age groups and the performance criterion employed in the current study. These questions need to be examined in future studies, possibly by varying bar task difficulty both between (by varying performance criterion) and within subjects (e.g., by varying individual threshold for angular disparity systematically, such that mismatch stimuli reflect angular difference at, below, and above threshold).

One limitation of the current study is that the participants were not screened for cognitive impairments or mood disorders using objective measures. Growing evidence shows potentially compensatory role of brain regions relevant to cognition-emotion interactions in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) during working memory tasks involving emotional distractors. In particular, previous studies found an increased activation in amygdala and prefrontal regions (Berger et al., 2015), as well as anterior cingulate (Burhan et al., 2016) in MCI patients relative to healthy controls, especially at higher levels of difficulty of working memory tasks performed in the context of task-irrelevant emotional material. Although none of our participants reported conditions known to impair cognitive and affective functioning (e.g., Parkinson’s or Alzheimer’s disease; stroke; epilepsy; multiple sclerosis; head injury or brain damage; depression or anxiety), a more detailed neuropsychological profiling based on objective instead of self-report measures could be a fruitful avenue for future research to better delineate impact of individuals differences on neural underpinnings of cognition-emotion interactions.

Conclusions

We found that when emotional material is task-relevant younger adults only discriminate emotional content from nonemotional content as evidenced in ERPs, whereas ERPs in older adults provide evidence for a selective bias towards positive material. Although processing of negative material did not differ by age, there also was no age-related differences in the magnitude of the positivity effect. Thus, the absence of age-related differences in processing of negative material combined with the positivity effect observed within the older group provides partial support to the claim that the well-known age-related positivity effect arises from an increased focus on positive material as suggested by SST (Carstensen et al., 1999), rather than a reduced negativity bias in older adults as suggested by ABM (Cacioppo et al., 2011).

When emotional material is task-irrelevant, ERP amplitudes for emotional and neutral images in the older group were virtually identical, suggesting that older adults are capable of inhibiting irrelevant emotional material over and above neutral material, even if this material is presented at fixation. These results are consistent with perceptual load theory (Lavie, 1995), which argues that when the task is sufficiently demanding, individuals prioritize task-relevant stimuli, resulting in effective rejection of task-irrelevant material. Younger adults showed larger suppression of emotional distractors relative to older adults, in the absence of age-related differences at baseline. More research is needed to examine the exact mechanism behind this age-related difference in task approach.