Technical progress during the last decades, particularly in the domains of communication technology and human–machine interactions, has tremendously increased dual-tasking requirements for the individual with regard to both quantity and quality. Similar to dual-tasking, stress has become an omnipresent aspect of modern life, and thus successful performing even under conditions of acute stress represents an everyday demand for more and more individuals. As a consequence, these similar trends are not only indicative of a need to address both issues, but also require an investigation of the specific link between acute stress and dual-task performance; this topic is pursued in the present study.

Whenever more than one task is performed at a time, the cognitive system faces additional demands that go beyond those of single-task processing (e.g., online order control, task-set separation, and task-component scheduling). Accordingly, theoretical models of dual-task performance (e.g., Logan & Gordon, 2001; Meyer & Kieras, 1997; Sigman & Dehaene, 2006) assume that simultaneous task performance is coordinated and realized by mechanisms of cognitive controlFootnote 1 and crucially depends on the efficient allocation of attentional resources (Navon & Miller, 2002; Tombu & Jolicœur, 2003). The strategic-response-deferment model (Meyer & Kieras, 1997; see also Logan & Gordon, 2001), for example, holds that secondary task processing is strategically delayed in order to meet the attentional requirements of the prioritized primary task processing. More specifically, dual-task performance depends, for example, on the ability to protect prioritized task processing from the interfering influences (i.e., crosstalk) of secondary task processing in order to avoid performance errors that not only impair the task at hand, but most likely would deteriorate performance within the entire dual-task context. Therefore, task shielding reflects cognitive control processes that enable the reduction of between-task interference by a task-specific separation of task-goal representations and the related stimulus–response (S–R) translation processes (e.g., Logan & Gordon, 2001)—for example, by increasing the activation of the prioritized task processing (Stelzel, Brandt, & Schubert, 2009) and/or by inhibiting competing task(−component) processes (see Koch, Gade, Schuch, & Philipp, 2010, for a review). Task shielding is of especially vital importance when both tasks are similar, and thus often represents a major challenge. It is therefore important to gain knowledge about how and which potential critical or beneficial factors jeopardize or determine the success of task shielding in dual-task performance. According to this reasoning, in the present study, we particularly focused on the question of how these cognitive control processes of task shielding are affected by acute stress experience.

The links between acute stress and cognitive control processes in dual-task performance are manifold. For example, early work in both laboratory and real-life settings has already suggested that prototypical dual-task demands (i.e., controlling an airplane) are related to increases in biological stress markers (e.g., Hale, Ellis, & Kratochvil, 1959; Pincus & Hoagland, 1943). In addition, the demands of performing two or more tasks at the same time represent high levels of task complexity—a factor that was found to also increase the probability that stress would reveal effects on cognitive performance (e.g., Oei, Everaerd, Elzinga, van Well, & Bermond, 2006; Schoofs, Wolf, & Smeets, 2009). Most importantly, both the cognitive control processes of task shielding in the service of successful dual-task performance and the physiological effects of acute stress share substantial neural commonalities, such as their relation to the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Regarding dual-task performance, neuroimaging studies have pointed to involvement of both the (dorso)lateral (e.g., Dux, Ivanoff, Asplund, & Marois, 2006; Marois & Ivanoff, 2005; Stelzel et al., 2009) and rostral (e.g., Koechlin, Basso, Pietrini, Panzer, & Grafman, 1999) PFC. Stelzel et al., for example, demonstrated that the dorsolateral PFC critically mediates shielding of the prioritized Task 1 (T1) by increasing activity in T1-relevant sensory areas, especially in conditions of simultaneous secondary Task 2 (T2) processing. At the same time, not only the cognitive control processes involved in dual-task performance, but also physiological stress effects, have been located in the PFC. When one is exposed to a stressor, two major stress axes get activated. First, stress induces an increase in sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activity and catecholamine release. High catecholamine levels have been found to decrease neuron firing rates in the PFC (cf. Ramos & Arnsten, 2007; Vijayraghavan, Wang, Birnbaum, Williams, & Arnsten, 2007). Second, and more slowly, stress triggers an activity increase in the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, which, in turn, induces the synthesis and release of glucocorticoids (primarily cortisol) into the bloodstream (e.g., de Kloet, Joels, & Holsboer, 2005). Under conditions of elevated glucocorticoid levels (e.g., stress), glucocorticoids primarily bind to glucocorticoid receptors (e.g., de Kloet & Reul, 1987) that are highly prevalent in the PFC (e.g., Perlman, Webster, Herman, Kleinman, & Weickert, 2007; Sanchez, Young, Plotsky, & Insel, 2000). Moreover, elevated glucocorticoid levels in conditions of acute stress have been found to alter PFC activity (e.g., Kern et al., 2008; Qin, Hermans, van Marle, Luo, & Fernandez, 2009).

Despite these established links on the physiological level, direct empirical evidence of how the specific PFC-related cognitive control functions involved in dual-tasking are influenced by acute stress is still missing. To our knowledge, only a few studies have demonstrated an impact of acute stress on the flexible implementation of task goals that enables a switch between tasks when environmental changes demand behavioral adaptation (e.g., Liston, McEwen, & Casey, 2009; Plessow, Kiesel, & Kirschbaum, 2012).Footnote 2 A recent study from our laboratory investigated the impact of acute stress on task (-goal) shielding in a selective-attention (i.e., Simon) task involving strong response conflict (Plessow, Fischer, Kirschbaum, & Goschke, 2011). The measure of interest was the trial-to-trial sequential adjustment of task shielding related to an encountered response conflict. The results revealed that, relative to controls, stressed individuals displayed tonically increased task shielding that was not only present in situations in which task shielding was required and functional (i.e., following conflict trials), but also occurred in situations in which task shielding was not indicated (i.e., following nonconflict trials). Put differently, this increased task shielding came at the cost of reduced cognitive flexibility in selectively adjusting the amount of shielding to varying trial-to-trial task requirements. Although this tonic increase in task shielding was interpreted as a compensatory mechanism in response to acute stress, to date, it remains unclear whether increases in task shielding represent the default compensatory mechanism that is the “natural” response of the cognitive system to an acute stress experience (see also Easterbrook’s cue-utilization hypothesis; Easterbrook, 1959) or whether stress might trigger a specific compensatory mechanism that entails the most economical resource demands in terms of “choosing” the most resource-efficient task-processing mode available. Indeed, in the selective-attention task of Plessow et al. (2011), the most parsimonious and resource-efficient task-processing strategy might entail a tonic blocking of task-irrelevant information in order to prevent interference, whereas a flexible trial-to-trial adjustment of task shielding to varying task demands (i.e., response conflict vs. no response conflict) might appear too risky, error-prone, and resource-consuming.

Investigating the effects of acute stress on task shielding in dual-task performance allowed us to directly test these two predictions. Even though dual-task research has provided evidence that a task-processing mode of strong task shielding represents the most favorable processing strategy (Logan & Gordon, 2001; Miller, Ulrich, & Rolke, 2009; Navon & Miller, 1987), as it minimizes between-task interference (Koch, 2009), this mode is also associated with increased mental effort, since it requires temporary inhibition of T2 processing that has to be resumed as soon as T1 conductance is finished (Lehle, Steinhauser, & Hübner, 2009). Accordingly, participants had rated conditions of strong task shielding as more demanding, had displayed an increased heart rate (a physiological marker of mental effort) as compared to conditions of weaker task shielding (Lehle et al., 2009), and had adopted a strategy of little task shielding when provided with free choice (Lehle & Hübner, 2009).

On this basis, in the present study, the following predictions were derived: If acute stress experience invariably triggers a default compensatory mechanism of tonically increased task shielding, the dual-task context would predict less between-task interference (crosstalk) for stressed individuals than for controls. In contrast, if acute stress experience results in a compensatory mechanism that is associated with the most parsimonious demands on the available resources, the dual-task context would predict the adoption of a resource-saving task-processing mode of reduced task shielding by stressed individuals, leading to increased between-task interference for stressed participants as compared to controls.

In addition to the main focus of testing these opposing predictions, we aimed to address two further questions. First, because previous findings regarding acute stress effects on cognitive control have often implied increased interindividual variance in the stress group as compared to the control group (e.g., Plessow et al., 2012; Qin et al., 2009), we additionally tested for individual-related modulators of stress effects on cognitive performance by applying the action–state orientation concept (e.g., Kuhl, 2000; Kuhl & Beckmann, 1994). According to this theory, the ability to detach from an unpleasant past experience (captured in the preoccupation dimension) is assumed to especially represent a trait with two manifestations—that is, action-oriented individuals will successfully omit recurring thoughts about unpleasant past events, and state-oriented individuals will get absorbed by them. Therefore, we aimed to determine whether participants with different markedness on the preoccupation scale differ in the extents to which acute psychosocial stress may affect their task shielding during dual-task performance. Finally, since the effects of acute psychosocial stress on task shielding during single-task conductance were found to be time-dependent (i.e., they only developed with increasing time lag to the stressor; Plessow et al., 2011), we measured dual-task performance in two parts, covering two different time intervals with regard to stress exposure and the organism’s response to it, in order to examine the time axis of potential stress-induced modulations of task shielding during dual-task performance (for a similar proceeding, see Plessow et al., 2012).

To test the outlined predictions with respect to acute stress and task shielding in dual-task performance, we administered acute stress by applying a well-established psychosocial stress-induction protocol (i.e., the Trier Social Stress Test [TSST]; Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993). The success of the stress induction was validated using biological and subjective stress markers (i.e., salivary α-amylase [sAA] and salivary cortisol, as markers of SNS and HPA-axis activity, respectively—see, e.g., Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1994; Nater & Rohleder, 2009; participants also filled out the German MDBF mood questionnaire to assess their current mental state; Steyer, Schwenkmezger, Notz, & Eid, 1997). To target cognitive control processes of task shielding in dual-task performance, the participants were instructed to perform two choice-reaction tasks in close temporal succession. To ensure maximum between-task interference (crosstalk), we used tasks with identical S–R rules (e.g., Fischer, Miller, & Schubert, 2007; Logan & Schulkind, 2000). More precisely, the participants were to categorize single-digit numbers as odd or even, first in T1 and then in T2. As a typical finding, categorization of the target stimulus for Task 1 (S1) is often affected by the simultaneous categorization of the target stimulus for Task 2 (S2), denoting the response-category match effect (e.g., Fischer et al., 2007a; Hommel, 1998; Logan & Schulkind, 2000; Miller, 2006; Schubert, Fischer, & Stelzel, 2008; Schuch & Koch, 2004; see Lien & Proctor, 2002, for an overview), which is especially detrimental when S2 calls for a different categorization than S1 does (e.g., S1 = odd and S2 = even). Therefore, the response-category match effect designates the amount of between-task interference, and thus the amount of task shielding (Fischer & Hommel, 2012). To reiterate, strong task shielding is associated with small response-category match effects and would be expected if increases in task shielding represent a default compensatory mechanism during acute stress. Conversely, weak task shielding is associated with large response-category match effects and would be expected if acute stress leads to task performance with priority to sparing the available resources.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-six volunteers (28 men, 18–30 years of age, mean age ± SD = 22.63 ± 2.79 years) took part in the study. All of them were healthy, non-medicated, and of normal weight (as indexed by the body-mass index [BMI], 17 < BMI < 28; mean BMI ± SD = 22.40 ± 2.50), and all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Fifty-three of the participants claimed right-handedness. None of the participants reported any acute or chronic stress condition. Due to the findings that both habitual smoking and oral contraceptive intake attenuate the physiological stress response, in terms of a smaller stress-related increase in free salivary cortisol (Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer, & Hellhammer, 1999; Rohleder & Kirschbaum, 2006), we tested only nonsmokers and female volunteers who did not use hormone-based birth control. All of the participants gave their written informed consent prior to inclusion in the study, in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki, and received financial compensation or course credit.

Stress induction and stress validation

The participants were randomly assigned to two experimental groups, with 14 men and 14 women in each group. The stress group was exposed to the TSST protocol (Kirschbaum et al., 1993). During this standardized stress-induction procedure, participants first received a verbal instruction about the upcoming tasks, followed by a 5-min anticipatory period. During the subsequent interaction phase, they had to deliver a public speech and perform a mental arithmetic task in front of a committee (for durations of 5 min each; total TSST time = 15 min + 5 min for taking the participant to the treatment room and instruction). The control group, however, underwent a standardized control situation that widely resembled the TSST protocol but omitted its stress-inducing features (for details, see Het, Rohleder, Schoofs, Kirschbaum, & Wolf, 2009).

Over the course of the session, at eight time points (i.e., 15, 5, and 1 min before and 1, 10, 20, 30, and 40 min following treatment), saliva samples were collected using Salivette sampling devices (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany). Via a quantitative enzyme-kinetic method, sAA activity was obtained (cf. Rohleder & Nater, 2009). Applying a chemiluminescence immunoassay (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany), free-cortisol levels were estimated. In order to additionally assess subjective stress experience, the MDBF mood questionnaire (Steyer et al., 1997) was employed, which displays the current mental state of an individual on three dimensions (i.e., good mood vs. bad mood, calmness vs. restlessness, and alertness vs. fatigue). This self-report measure was implemented five times (i.e., at 15 and 1 min prior to and 1, 20, and 40 min after treatment) simultaneously with the saliva sampling, with the solitary exception of measurement time point −1 min. At this point, saliva sampling took place prior to the instruction for the upcoming treatment, whereas the MDBF was to be answered only immediately after the treatment introduction. As a consequence, the participants gave account of their current mental state while already anticipating the upcoming treatment situation (i.e., TSST or standardized control situation).

Dual task

In the present study, we implemented a dual-task paradigm that was closely modeled after the study by Logan and Schulkind (2000) and that has proven a reliable tool for the investigation of task shielding in dual-task performance (e.g., Fischer & Hommel, 2012; Fischer et al., 2007a; Oriet, Tombu, & Jolicœur, 2005). In this paradigm, participants performed the same single-digit categorization in Tasks 1 and 2 to maximize the feature overlap between tasks. The measure of interest was Task 1 performance (i.e., error rates and response times [RTs]).

Two target stimuli (i.e., the digits 1–9, except for 5), with a size of 2–3 mm horizontally and 5 mm vertically, were presented one above the other in white against black on a 17-in. monitor of an IBM-compatible personal computer. The upper digit denoted the target stimulus for Task 1 (S1), whereas the lower digit represented the target stimulus for Task 2 (S2). The participants were instructed to categorize both stimuli with regard to their parity by providing manual responses on a standard QWERTZ keyboard. For Task 1, the participants responded with the right index finger (“AltGr” key) and the middle finger (“.” key) to odd and even targets, respectively. For Task 2, homologously, odd targets had to be answered with the left index finger (“Alt” key), whereas even targets required a left middle finger response (“Y” key).

Each trial started with the presentation of a fixation display consisting of four horizontal lines (2.5 mm in length, two 4.5 mm above and two 4.5 mm below screen center, the upper and lower lines each displaced 5 mm to the left and right of a notional line through screen center) indicating the space between a left and a right line on the upper and the lower parts of the display as presentation locations for the upcoming S1 and S2, respectively (total fixation display size: 12 × 14 mm, resulting in a visual angle of 1.15º × 1.34º at a viewing distance of 60 cm). After 500 ms, S1 was displayed at the upper target-stimulus location. With a stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) of 50, 150, or 900 ms, S2 was added at the lower target-stimulus location. The participants were instructed to first respond as quickly and accurately as possible to S1, and only subsequently to respond also as quickly and accurately as possible to S2 (Task 1 priority instruction). Furthermore, the instructions stressed that participants refrain from delaying their response to S1. The delayed S2 onset facilitated the prioritization of Task 1 processing (in addition to the instructions) and allowed for a systematic manipulation of the overlap (i.e., the task load) of dual-task processing. Both stimuli were displayed for 1,000 ms (plus SOA, for S1), followed by 800 ms of blank screen, providing a total response window of 1,800 ms from S2 onset. Within all trials, feedback was provided for 300 ms. If both answers were given correctly, the word “richtig” (correct) was displayed. If at least one response was erroneous, “falsch” (false) was reported. If less than two answers were given within the response window, “zu langsam” (too slow) was presented. Following a random blank-screen interval between 100 and 1,000 ms, the next trial started. The stimulus presentation and data recording were implemented using Presentation software (Version 0.71; Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, USA).

Action–state orientation

To assess the disposition toward action–state orientation in preoccupation situations, all participants completed the action-control scale (ACS-90; Kuhl, 1994). According to the total score on the preoccupation subscale, the ACS allowed us to differentiate individuals with a disposition toward state orientation (<5) from individuals who primarily orient themselves toward their current action (≥5) following on an unpleasant experience. This resulted in 12 state-oriented and 16 action-oriented individuals within the stress group and 11 state-oriented and 17 action-oriented volunteers in the control group, χ 2(1, N = 56) = 0.07, p = .79.

Procedure

We first trained the cognitive task (total number of training trials = 132) to familiarize participants with the dual-task procedure, and thereby to minimize practice and learning effects during posttreatment cognitive testing. At 30 min after arrival, the participants underwent the treatment (i.e., TSST or standardized control situation). At 5 min after treatment completion, the first part of cognitive testing started (five blocks comprising 48 trials each = 240 trials). The second part of the dual task followed with a time lag of 25 min from the end of treatment (five blocks of 48 trials each = 240 trials; total number of test trials = 480, total session duration = 100 min).

Sessions were scheduled between 11:30 a.m. and 8 p.m. Due to evidence for an influence of instantaneous food and caffeine intake on the cortisol stress response assessed via saliva (Gonzalez-Bono, Rohleder, Hellhammer, Salvador, & Kirschbaum, 2002; Lovallo, Farag, Vincent, Thomas, & Wilson, 2006), participants were asked to refrain from eating and from consuming sugary or caffeinated beverages 2 h prior to their appointed time. Since acute glucose availability is assumed to be essential for a stress-related HPA-axis activity increase (Kirschbaum et al., 1997), all participants received 200 ml of grape juice at the beginning of the session to elevate their blood glucose levels and to allow these to converge across individuals.

Data analysis

In order to address changes in individual physiological and subjective stress levels over the course of the session, repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs), comprising the within-subjects factor Measurement Time Point (eight or five levels, respectively) and the between-subjects factor Stress (stress vs. no stress) were conducted on sAA, cortisol, and the total scores on the three MDBF dimensions. In preparation for the statistical analyses (i.e., to ensure compliance with the requirements of general-linear-model-based procedures in terms of Gaussian distribution), the sAA and cortisol data were first logarithmized to base 10. Dual-task performance was analyzed by entering the within-subjects factors Response-Category Match (mismatch vs. match), SOA (50 vs. 150 vs. 900 ms), and Part (first vs. second), as well as the between-subjects factors Stress (stress vs. no stress) and Action–State Orientation (action-oriented vs. state-oriented) into repeated measures ANOVAs on the error rates and mean RTs in Task 1. To eliminate the possibility of perceptual matching, trials with identical target stimuli in both tasks (12.65 %) were excluded. For the RT analysis only, error trials (9.03 %) and RTs differing by more than 2.5 standard deviations from the means of the individual-cell mean RTs in both tasks (3.37 %) were also omitted. Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were applied where appropriate.

Results

Stress response

Neuroendocrine measures

Stressed individuals and controls differed in their cortisol time courses, F(7, 378) = 32.42, p < .001, η 2p = .38. While no group differences occurred prior to TSST and the standardized control situation, respectively, all ps ≥ .29, following treatment the stress group constantly displayed higher cortisol levels than did the control group: 1 min, t(54) = 2.31, p < .05; 10 min, t(54) = 4.67, p < .001; 20 min, t(54) = 4.91, p < .001; 30 min, t(54) = 4.28, p < .001; and 40 min, t(54) = 3.70, p < .01. For sAA, the Measurement Time Point × Stress interaction did not reach significance, F(7, 378) = 1.88, p = .11, η 2p = .03. Post-hoc comparisons for the single measurement time points (parallel to the cortisol-level analysis) revealed that, relative to controls, stressed individuals showed higher sAA activity solely immediately after treatment (1 min), t(54) = 2.42, p < .05, all other ps ≥ .14 (Fig. 1 top).

Fig. 1
figure 1

(Top) Mean levels of salivary α-amylase (sAA) and salivary cortisol as a function of time (minutes before or after treatment) for the stress group and the control group. (Bottom) Error rates in Task 1 as a function of response-category match (mismatch vs. match) for the stress group and the control group and for Dual-Task Parts 1 and 2. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001

Mental state

The MDBF data analyses revealed different time courses for the stress group and the control group with respect to the two dimensions of good mood versus bad mood and calmness versus restlessness, F(4, 216) = 3.62, p < .05, η 2p = .06, and F(4, 216) = 4.68, p < .01, η 2p = .08. More precisely, stressed participants displayed worse mood directly after treatment (1 min), relative to controls, t(54) = 2.73, p < .01, and a trend toward worse mood after receiving the treatment instructions but prior to the actual treatment (−1 min), t(54) = 1.73, p = .09 (all other ps ≥ .20). Immediately following treatment, the volunteers in the stress group also reported more restlessness than did controls, t(54) = 2.62, p < .05, whereas no group differences regarding calmness versus restlessness were found for all other measurement time points, ps ≥ .14. Fatigue increased throughout the session, F(4, 216) = 26.29, p < .001, η 2p = .33, developing equally in both the stress and control group, F(4, 216) = 1.13, p = .33, η 2p = .02. The mean fatigue levels, too, were similar in both groups, F < 1 (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mental state measured on the three dimensions good mood versus bad mood, calmness versus restlessness, and alertness versus fatigue, assessed by the MDBF mood questionnaire (Steyer et al., 1997), as a function of time (minutes before or after treatment) for the stress group and the control group. The mental-state measurement time point −1 min took place directly after the instruction for the subsequent treatment (i.e., Trier Social Stress Test or standardized control situation), within the anticipatory period. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. * p < .05. ** p < .01

Cognitive performance

The results of Task 1 performance (error rates and RTs) are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 1 (bottom).Footnote 3

Table 1 Error rates (%) and response times (RTs) in Task 1 for all combinations of response-category match and stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) for the stress group and the control group overall, as well as separately for Dual-Task Parts 1 and 2

Error rates

Dual-task performance

Significant main effects were found for both response-category match (mismatch, 6.58 %; match, 3.89 %), F(1, 52) = 49.40, p < .001, η 2p = .49, and SOA (50 ms, 6.12 %; 150 ms, 5.46 %; 900 ms, 4.05 %), F(2, 104) = 10.06, p < .001, η 2p = .16. Furthermore, the effect of response-category match decreased with increasing SOA (50 ms, 4.81 %; 150 ms, 3.41 %; 900 ms, –0.16 %), F(2, 104) = 20.14, p < .001, η 2p = .28.

Effects of acute psychosocial stress and action–state orientation on dual-task performance

Most important for the present study, the response-category match effect was modulated by stress, F(1, 52) = 5.10, p < .05, η 2p = .09. More precisely, stressed participants displayed a larger response-category match effect (3.55 %) than did controls (1.82 %). Post-hoc t tests with independent measures showed that this increased response-category match effect primarily originated from stress-related error rate increases in response-category mismatch trials (stress, 8.02 %; no stress, 5.14 %), t(54) = 2.01, p < .05 (one-tailed). In contrast, no group difference was found for response-category match trials, t(54) = 1.02, p = .31. In order to test whether the observed effect of stress on the response-category match effect on the group-comparison level was also evident on the individual level, we correlated the total increase in neuroendocrine measures following treatmentFootnote 4 with the amount of between-task interference (i.e., the response-category match effect) of each individual. This led to a significant correlation between the cumulative increase in salivary cortisol following treatment and the response-category match effect, rs(56) = .32, p < .05. The larger the total increase in salivary cortisol, the more pronounced the response-category match effect. In contrast, treatment-related changes in sAA did not correlate with the response-category match effect, rs(56) = −.07, p = .62.

A larger response-category match effect was also found for state-oriented individuals (3.58 %) than for action-oriented participants (1.79 %; Fig. 3), which is reflected in the interaction between Response-Category Match and action–state orientation, F(1, 52) = 5.49, p < .05, η 2p = .10. Importantly, the effects of stress and action–state orientation on the response-category match effect did not interact, F(1, 52) = 1.54, p = .22, η 2p = .03. The aforementioned decrease of error rates with increasing SOA seemed slightly pronounced in the stress group (50 ms, 7.63 %; 150 ms, 6.70 %; 900 ms, 4.40 %) as compared to the control group (50 ms, 4.77 %; 150 ms, 4.23 %; 900 ms, 3.70 %). This observation, however, fell just short of significance, F(2, 104) = 2.78, p = .07, η 2p = .05. At the same time, the decrease of the response-category match effect with increasing SOA was unaffected by stress, F(2, 104) = 1.56, p = .22, η 2p = .03. Also, neither the SOA effect nor the Response-Category Match × SOA interaction was affected by action–state orientation, either alone or combined with stress, ps ≥ .15. Overall, the mean error rates were numerically larger in stressed individuals (6.24 %) than in controls (4.23 %). This difference, however, only approached significance, F(1, 52) = 3.29, p = .08, η 2p = .06. No main effect of action–state orientation on error rates was revealed, F(1, 52) = 2.42, p = .13, η 2p = .04. Furthermore, stress and action–state orientation did not interact, F(1, 52) = 1.21, p = .28, η 2p = .02.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Error rates in Task 1 as a function of response-category match (mismatch vs. match) for state-oriented and action-oriented individuals. Error bars represent standard errors of the means

To test whether the observed effects depended on gender, we repeated the original ANOVA with all a priori factors and the additional between-subjects factor Gender. The results showed no modulation by gender of the revealed effects of stress and action–state orientation on the response-category match effect, both Fs < 1. Moreover, gender interacted neither with stress alone nor with any factorial combination including stress, all ps ≥ .16.

Comparison of the two parts

Overall, the mean error rate decreased from Dual-Task Part 1 (5.66 %) to Dual-Task Part 2 (4.82 %), F(1, 52) = 5.85, p < .05, η 2p = .10. In contrast, the effects of both response-category match and SOA remained unchanged, Fs < 1, as did the reduction of the response-category match effect with increasing SOA, F(2, 104) = 1.79, p = .17, η 2p = .03. The modulating influences of stress and action–state orientation on the response-category match effect did not differ between the dual-task parts, both Fs < 1. No further interactions of dual-task part with one or more of the other factors reached significance, all ps ≥ 09.

Yet, in order to draw clear conclusions regarding the occurrence intervals of a reliable stress impact on the response-category match effect (Plessow et al., 2011), it seems inevitable that we should additionally demonstrate that the stress impact on the response-category match effect can be substantiated by the appropriate inference statistics when separately analyzing Dual-Task Parts 1 and 2 (see also Fig. 1). Following this reasoning, we repeated the original ANOVA (excluding the within-subjects factor Part) for both cognitive-testing parts separately. For the first part of the dual task, the response-category match effect was only numerically larger for the stress group (3.37 %) than for the control group (2.04 %), but it was not statistically reliable, F(1, 52) = 2.06, p = .16, η 2p = .04. The main effect of stress on error rates, with larger error rates for stressed individuals (6.88 %) than for controls (4.43 %) in Dual-Task Part 1 was close to significance, F(1, 52) = 3.84, p = .06, η 2p = .07. Within the second part of the dual task, the response-category match effect was larger in the stress group (3.73 %) than in the control group (1.61 %), F(1, 52) = 6.11, p < .05, η 2p = .11. Equivalently to the stress impact on the overall response-category match effect, this difference between the stress and control group was driven by larger error rates in response-category mismatch trials for stressed participants (7.47 %) than for controls (4.84 %), t(54) = 1.92, p < .05 (one-tailed). Error rates did not differ between the groups in response-category match trials, t < 1. At the same time, the main effect of stress on error rates was not significant, F(1, 52) = 2.16, p = .15, η 2p = .04 (Fig. 1 bottom).

RTs

Dual-task performance

The RT analysis revealed a significant main effect of response-category match (mismatch, 774 ms; match, 722 ms), F(1, 52) = 114.29, p < .001, η 2p = .69. Moreover, the response-category match effect decreased with increasing SOA (50 ms, 94 ms; 150 ms, 58 ms; 900 ms, 4 ms), F(2, 104) = 46.54, p < .001, η 2p = .47. SOA alone did not affect RTs, F < 1.

Effects of acute psychosocial stress and action–state orientation on dual-task performance

There was only a trend toward larger overall mean RTs in stressed participants (781 ms) than in controls (715 ms), F(1, 52) = 2.77, p = .10, η 2p = .05. Similarly, the response-category match effect was numerically, but not statistically, larger for the stress group (58 ms) than for the control group (46 ms), F(1, 52) = 1.44, p = .24, η 2p = .03, and was neither affected by action–state orientation nor by the combination of both factors, Fs < 1.

Comparison of the two parts

RTs were not significantly affected by part, F(1, 52) = 1.70, p = .20, η 2p = .03. However, a significant interaction between part and stress was found, F(1, 52) = 6.10, p < .05, η 2p = .11. Whereas the stress group did not differ between Dual-Task Parts 1 and 2 (788 and 780 ms, respectively), t(27) = 1.02, p = .32, the RTs of the control group were slower in Dual-Task Part 2 (731 ms) than in Dual-Task Part 1 (709 ms), t(27) = 2.70, p < .05. This effect seemed especially pronounced at the long SOA, resulting in the close-to-significant interaction SOA × Part × Stress, F(2, 104) = 3.31, p = .06, η 2p = .06 (see also Table 1). Regarding action–state orientation, state-oriented individuals displayed a trend toward larger RTs in the second (761 ms) than in the first part of the dual task (744 ms), while numerical inspection revealed no such difference for action-oriented individuals (Part 1, 746 ms; Part 2, 743 ms), F(1, 52) = 3.01, p = .09, η 2p = .06. State-oriented individuals showed an increase in RTs from the first to the second cognitive-testing part primarily for the long SOA, which is reflected in a significant three-way interaction between SOA, part, and action–state orientation, F(2, 104) = 3.82, p < .05, η 2p = .07. Importantly, the four-way interaction SOA × Part × Stress × Action–State Orientation was not significant, F(2, 104) = 2.26, p = .13, η 2p = .04. Independent of the between-subjects factors, there was a trend toward a larger response-category match effect in Dual-Task Part 2 (58 ms) as compared to Part 1 (47 ms), F(1, 52) = 3.68, p = .06, η 2p = .07. No further significant interactions occurred, all ps ≥ .20.

General quality of dual-task performance

The quality of dual-task performance is often reported in total performance measures such as the added RTs of both tasks (e.g., Miller et al., 2009). In the present study, we did not find significant effects in total error rates (stress, 9.72 %; no stress, 8.34 %), t < 1, or total RTs (stress, 1,571 ms; no stress, 1,468 ms), t(54) = 1.59, p = .12.

Discussion

The present study investigated the impact of acute psychosocial stress on cognitive control processes of task shielding in a dual-task context. Following a previous single-task study that showed tonically increased levels of task shielding under acute psychosocial stress (Plessow et al., 2011), we aimed at testing whether these tonically increased levels of task shielding represent a default compensatory mechanism that is reflexively triggered by acute stress experience or whether acute stress results in adopting the most parsimonious task-processing mode in order to spare mental resources.

These assumptions are testable in dual-task performance, in which a task-processing mode with increased levels of task shielding is also concomitant with increased mental effort and resource load (Lehle et al., 2009). Therefore, in a dual-task context, a default compensatory mechanism of increased task shielding would reduce between-task interference (i.e., lead to a smaller response-category match effect) under acute stress and should be observable even when this mechanism does not reflect the resource-conserving task-processing mode. If the response to an acute stress experience, however, relies on the adoption of a resource-conserving task-processing mode, within a dual-task situation, task shielding should be less strong under conditions of acute stress, resulting in increased between-task interference (i.e., a larger response-category match effect) for stressed individuals than for controls.

The results are straightforward: First, as expected, the applied stress-induction protocol (i.e., TSST) reliably evoked the typical two-component physiological stress response, comprising an increase in both SNS and HPA-axis activity, as indicated by increased sAA activity and elevated salivary cortisol levels in the stress group as compared to the control group at different time points following treatment. Second, the stress group displayed a larger response-category match effect in Task 1, and thus less task shielding than the control group. On an individual level, this was expressed by a significant correlation between the individual treatment-related changes in cortisol levels as a marker for HPA-axis activity and the response-category match effect (see below for a more detailed discussion).

These findings of reduced task shielding in dual-task performance under conditions of acute stress contradict the assumption of a unitary consequence of acute stress on cognitive control processes in terms of increased task shielding. This hypothesis can be traced to early theoretical work suggesting that stress narrows attention and internal processing (Easterbrook, 1959), and thus predicting increased rather than reduced task shielding. Easterbrook’s idea of stress-induced attentional focusing has received recent empirical support—for example, in demonstrations of reduced interference from competing automatic response tendencies when performing a Stroop task under potentially stressful conditions (e.g., Chajut & Algom, 2003; Huguet, Galvaing, Monteil, & Dumas, 1999), as well as in our own previous work, in which we showed that an acute stress experience results in tonically increased task shielding at the cost of reduced flexibility in a selective-attention task (Plessow et al., 2011).

The apparently conflicting findings of the present study and the assumption of increased task shielding as a compensatory mechanism under stress (in line with Easterbrook’s narrowed-attention account) can be reconciled with our alternative hypothesis, in which we assume that acute stress triggers the adoption of a task-processing mode that is less resource-demanding. As mentioned earlier, in a single-task situation, a tonic increase in task shielding seems to represent a less hazardous and resource-demanding task-processing mode that is favored over the usual flexible trial-to-trial adaptation of task shielding to response-conflict occurrence under acute stress (Plessow et al., 2011). In a dual-task context, however, the less resource-consuming task-processing mode is associated with reduced task shielding that increases parallel processing (Lehle et al., 2009)—for example, by relaxing between-task inhibition (Koch et al., 2010).Footnote 5

The adoption of a task-processing mode that saves available resources under stress is in line with ideas highlighting the relevance of additional stress-coping mechanisms occupying resources that are consequently not available for other resource-dependent cognitive processes (e.g., Kahneman, 1973; Keinan, Friedland, Kahneman, & Roth, 1999). In these terms, we may conclude that stressed individuals adopted a resource-saving mode of dual-task processing. Thereby, the question of whether limited resources coercively necessitated the resource-sparing task-performance mode or whether its adoption was the result of resource reallocation processes triggered by acute stress experience (e.g., as proposed in the cognitive–energetical framework of Hockey, 1997), and thus of a “choice” made between two possible task-processing modes, remains to be answered.

Arnsten (2009) has gone even further by concluding that “acute uncontrollable stress impairs PFC-mediated cognitive functions in humans and animals and switches the control of behaviour and emotion to more primitive brain circuits” (Arnsten, 2009, p. 412). On a related note, Schwabe, Wolf, and Oitzl (2010) stated that stress experience leads to a stronger recourse to neostriatum-dependent rigid “habit” memory and a reduction in the use of hippocampus- and PFC-dependent flexible “cognitive” memory.

Supporting the assumption of impaired PFC-dependent cognitive functions under stress, studies have revealed impairments in suppressing ongoing responses (Scholz et al., 2009), in the flexible implementation of task goals (e.g., Liston et al., 2009; Plessow et al., 2012), and in working memory processes serving to maintain task-relevant information (e.g., Elzinga & Roelofs, 2005; Oei et al., 2006). On a more general level, stress was found to reduce cognitive flexibility during creative-thinking and problem-solving tasks (Alexander, Hillier, Smith, Tivarus, & Beversdorf, 2007), as well as the use of negative feedback (Petzold, Plessow, Goschke, & Kirschbaum, 2010).

In our view, the results of the present study do not support the assumption of a global impairment in PFC-mediated cognitive functions. Even though stressed participants were exposed to a stress-induction method (TSST) that is perceived as ego-threatening and uncontrollable (cf. Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004), they were, nevertheless, able to maintain overall high dual-task performance levels (as indicated, for example, by a total performance level that did not significantly differ from that of the controls). Therefore, in the context of dual tasks, we conclude that acute stress induces a shift in the recruitment of different cognitive control processes and task-processing modes (see also Plessow et al., 2011) rather than a shift away from the involvement of cognitive control. Thereby, choosing the less resource-demanding task-processing mode may also result in action control on the basis of mostly automatized S–R links (e.g., Schwabe et al., 2010). Yet the strong assumption of a loss of prefrontal regulation under conditions of acute stress may, at least in the face of successful dual-task performance, not be universally valid.

The assumption that acute stress results in the adoption of a resource-saving task-processing mode (i.e., reduced task shielding) is of high relevance for theories on dual-tasking and seems to support theoretical notions of resource allocation in models of capacity sharing (e.g., Navon & Miller, 2002; Tombu & Jolicœur, 2003). According to the task demands and the situational context, available cognitive resources are flexibly allocated between tasks to ensure the most efficient dual-task performance. A threatening acute stress experience seems to draw on resources that are also required by the cognitive control mechanisms enabling efficient prioritized task shielding. Hence, the adaptation of a task-processing mode that is economical in its requirements of cognitive resources can be interpreted as a flexible adjustment of strategic dual-task processing according to parameters of the given situation (Logan & Gordon, 2001; Meyer & Kieras, 1997). In these terms, the finding that the amount of task shielding, and thus the shift to a more parallel task-processing mode (cf. Lehle & Hübner, 2009), is adaptively adjusted according to situational context parameters highlights the role of control processes in dual-task coordination and is difficult to reconcile with traditional postulations of a structural and immutable bottleneck (e.g., Pashler, 1994).

As noted above, the observation of a stress-related decrease in task shielding on the group level was further substantiated on the individual level by a significant correlation between the total increase in salivary cortisol after treatment and the response-category match effect. Thereby, a more pronounced response-category match effect indicating less task shielding was found to be associated with larger increases in salivary cortisol as a marker for HPA-axis activity (but not with treatment-related increases in sAA activity). The assumption of an association between the HPA stress response and the observed stress-related behavioral changes gets further support from the fact that an increased response-category match effect in the stress group relative to the control group was only numerically observed in the first part of the dual task (i.e., 5–20 min after the end of treatment) but was statistically reliable in the second part (i.e., 25–40 min following treatment, during the ongoing HPA stress response but in the absence of stress-related alterations in SNS activity; see also Plessow et al., 2011). Even though this does not preclude the SNS-related stress response from playing a pivotal role in the development of acute stress effects on the investigated cognitive control processes, it strongly highlights the relevance of the HPA stress response for these findings. In addition, at those time points at which the effect occurred, the groups did not differ with regard to their subjective stress experience and/or exertion (e.g., levels of mood, arousal, and fatigue).

A further interesting aspect of the present findings is that stress effects on the cognitive control processes of task shielding were predominantly evident in error rates. Although similar patterns of increased response-category match effects under stress were found numerically for the RTs of T1 and T2, in neither RT did stress significantly affect the response-category match effect. Most importantly, however, our data do not support an interpretation by means of an unspecific performance criterion shift (i.e., speed–accuracy trade-off) in terms of premature responding. It should also be noted that stress effects occurring exclusively in error rates are rather common and have been repeatedly reported by several work groups (e.g., Hsu, Garside, Massey, & McAllister-Williams, 2003; Plessow et al., 2011; Plessow et al., 2012). Therefore, we believe that this is a systematic effect which is not incidental. Even though a conclusive explanation of this selective influence is still warranted, at least for the present study, we can speculate that one reason why stress effects might be “easier” to obtain in error data could be due to stress effects being especially evident in response-category mismatch trials. Eliminating a larger number of erroneous trials in this condition might have lowered the mean RT level, as these RTs would have potentially been slow trials if the participants had been able to execute the correct response (see also Fischer, Schubert, & Liepelt, 2007).

To acknowledge potential gender effects in the interaction between acute stress and cognitive functions (e.g., Smeets, Dziobek, & Wolf, 2009; Wolf, Schommer, Hellhammer, McEwen, & Kirschbaum, 2001), we purposely recruited 50 % male and 50 % female volunteers who were equally allocated to the stress and control group. Importantly, the observed impact of acute psychosocial stress on processes of cognitive control was not modulated by gender (see also Plessow et al., 2011; Plessow et al., 2012), which suggests stable and universal influences that reveal themselves in a gender-nonspecific manner. Furthermore, in the present study, we accounted for individual dispositions in action–state orientation as a potential modulator of acute psychosocial stress effects on cognitive control parameters by including data from the action-control scale (Kuhl, 1994), on the basis of which individuals are classified as being more action-oriented versus state-oriented, differing with regard to their levels of assumed decisiveness, flexibility in adapting to new situations, and ability to detach from previous unpleasant experiences (cf. Kuhl & Beckmann, 1994). We found no differences between action-oriented and state-oriented individuals in the extents to which acute psychosocial stress affected their task shielding. Hence, we suppose that stress-related reduced task shielding in dual-task performance is not due to higher levels of preoccupation (e.g., sustained rumination) with the unpleasant stress experience (as would be typical for individuals with a disposition toward state orientation, but not for action-oriented participants). Interestingly, the finding of overall increased response-category match effects for state-oriented relative to action-oriented participants, however, might point to either a general predisposition toward decreased task-shielding capability (and/or toward more shared representations of the individual tasks within the dual-task context) in individuals with a disposition toward state orientation.

In summary, with the present study, we have provided evidence that acute psychosocial stress affects the cognitive control processes specifically involved in dual-task performance. More precisely, we demonstrated that acute stress experience results in an adaptation of control settings in a dual-task situation in terms of a shift toward more parallel processing (reduced task shielding). This shows that two pivotal everyday demands—successful performance under stress conditions and simultaneous conductance of multiple tasks—cannot be regarded as independent, but rather interact strongly in determining the performance outcome. Subsequent research will be needed to extend the present research by targeting further cognitive control mechanisms that are also essential for successful dual-task performance (e.g., online order control; Luria & Meiran, 2003) and to advance our understanding of the underlying biological mediators involved.