A plethora of everyday life situations involves having more than one task at hand (i.e., multitasking; Koch, Poljac, Müller, & Kiesel, 2018). One special case of multitasking situations is having to work on two tasks simultaneously (i.e., dual tasking). In a laboratory setting, participants in dual-task situations are often required to respond to two tasks in rapid succession and the general finding is that performance for both tasks suffers compared to a single-task situation (Pashler, 1984; 1994; Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2004), even when only one overt response per trial is required and the other task requires no response (Miller & Durst, 2014; 2015). Another well-documented finding is that when the second task (T2) characteristics (e.g., stimuli or responses) are incompatible with the characteristics of the first task (T1), this typically leads to interference, and is often termed a backward crosstalk effect. In dual-tasking studies, it has often been shown that characteristics of T2 can influence first task performance (e.g., Caessens, Hommel, Reynvoet, & Vandergoten, 2004; Ellenbogen & Meiran, 2008; Hommel, 1998; Huestegge, Pieczykolan, & Janczyk, 2018; Janczyk, Renas, & Durst, 2018; Ko & Miller, 2014; Lien & Proctor, 2000; Lien, Ruthruff, Hsieh, & Yu, 2007; Logan & Schulkind, 2000; Miller, 2006; Miller & Durst, 2015; Navon & Miller, 1987).

Backward crosstalk effects

Navon and Miller (1987) found that processing of two tasks rarely occurs independently, and this potentially allows for interference from one task to the other. Most interestingly, they found that characteristics of a task performed later could influence performance of a task performed earlier, which might be termed a “backward crosstalk effect” (BCE). In a seminal study of BCEs, Hommel (1998) used stimuli with two dimensions (e.g., a colored letter), and the two tasks required sequentially responding to both dimensions. One central finding of this study was that when the response to the second task (R2) was compatible with the response to the first task (R1) (e.g., first having to respond with the left hand to the stimulus’ color, R1, and then responding with a vocal “left” to the stimulus’ identity, R2), responses to the first task were faster than when R2 was incompatible with R1 (e.g., first having to respond with the left hand as R1 and the verbally responding “right” as R2). The general finding of this and other types of backward crosstalk has been replicated and extended in various studies (e.g., Hommel and Eglau, 2002; Janczyk, 2016; Logan & Schulkind, 2000).

One reason why the existence of BCEs in dual-task situations has puzzled researchers is that it challenges the assumption that only one stimulus-response (S-R) rule can be active at a time during the response selection stage of processing, which is a key assumption of response selection bottleneck (RSB) models (e.g., Pashler, 1994). Hommel (1998) argued that S-R translation can occur automatically and is a distinct stage from final response selection—and therefore also allows for parallel activation of a given response by more than one S-R rule at a time. A different theoretical account integrates BCEs into resource sharing models, such as the EPIC model by (Meyer and Kieras, 1997a; 1997b). Specifically, the EPIC model (along with other resource sharing models) has no limitation in how many S-R rules can be active at a time—even for response selection. Thus, the main difference in accounting for BCEs in RSB models and resource sharing models is that in the former, responses are automatically activated upon stimulus presentation as a process which is distinct from response selection and in the latter, activation of incompatible responses can occur during the response selection stage.

Besides interference effects of task-relevant characteristics (e.g., interfering activation of different response sets of the second task), it has also been shown that task-irrelevant stimulus and response features can produce crosstalk. For instance, Miller and Alderton (2006) found that instructed response force in the second task affected the response force to a first task, without any instructions to modulate response force to T1. Moreover, Ruiz Fernández and Ulrich (2010) found that T2 movement distance to execute the second response influenced the RTs to the first task, even though T1 just required a key press—not a ballistic movement.

The present experiments

The present experiments aimed at disentangling the separate contributions of two different sources of compatibility to the BCEs observed in paradigms with common response sets for T1 and T2. Our general approach followed a logic similar to that of Janczyk, Pfister, Hommel, and Kunde (2014). In their experiments, they aimed at disentangling response compatibility effects from action effect compatibility effects. To this end, they used a dual-task paradigm where each task was mapped to one hand. Specifically, participants were always asked to first respond to T1 and subsequently to T2, resembling a psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, but without varying the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between S1 and S2. Importantly, in two conditions, the response to T2 could result in either compatible or incompatible action effects (i.e., a light appearing or a virtual lever moving toward either the same or opposite direction as the required response side). Their main finding was that the effect of R2−R1 compatibility (e.g., left-left responses as compatible) was significantly decreased—and even descriptively reversed (i.e., negative R2−R1 compatibility effect)—if the associated action effects were incompatible. Thus, they interpreted their findings as indicating that action effect compatibility plays a considerable role in the emergence of BCEs.

The present study aims at an analogous investigation of possibly separate contributions of stimulus and response compatibility to the BCE. Two major—not entirely mutually exclusive—possible accounts of BCEs are: (a) stimulus-based compatibility, i.e., application of the T1 rule to S2 creates interference if the stimulus of T2 is associated with an incompatible response of T1 (S2−R1 compatibility), and (b) response-based compatibility, i.e., the response to T2 is activated in time and influences T1 processing (R2−R1 compatibility). Crucially, the S2−R1 compatibility account of BCEs is in principle compatible with bottleneck models, as only one response is being selected at a time, even though multiple stimuli are driving that response—with only one S-R rule being active at that time. Moreover, S2−R1 compatibility could in principle also just be a case of a single response being driven by more than one source of information, as is typically observed in the flanker task (B. A. Eriksen & Eriksen 1974) and in experiments investigating coactivation (e.g., Miller, 1982). On the other hand, the R2−R1 compatibility account, which is presumably mediated by motor activation (e.g., Ko & Miller, 2014; Lien et al., 2007; Miller, 2017), is incompatible with pure RSB models of dual-task processing, since multiple S-R rules are being used to activate responses simultaneously.

To discriminate between these accounts, we embedded the flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) within a dual-task paradigm by assigning an S-R rule not just for the center letter, but also for the flankers. In the classic flanker task, participants are presented a string of letters (e.g., KKSKK) and are instructed to only respond to the identity of the center letter, ignoring the (irrelevant) identity of the flanker letters. In this task, the flanker letters usually produce compatibility effects. Specifically, when the flanker letters are assigned to a different response than the center letter, responses are slower than when the flanker letters are assigned to the same response as the center letter (e.g., B. A. Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; C. W. Eriksen & Schultz, 1979; Miller, 1991).

In our present experiments, we used the general flanker set-up to investigate the source of dual-task BCEs. To separate S2−R1 compatibility from R2−R1 compatibility as potential sources of the BCE, it seems necessary to have comparable stimuli and responses for the two tasks, and using letter tasks as T1 and T2 in a flanker dual-task setup seems ideal for this purpose. In Experiment 1, we used a prioritized processing (PP) paradigm (Miller & Durst, 2014), in which participants are asked to first respond to the center letter as quickly and accurately as possible and to respond to the flanker letters only if the center letter requires a no-go response. In Experiment 2, we embedded the flanker task in a PRP paradigm (Telford, 1931; Welford, 1952) where participants were always asked to first respond to the center letters and then respond to the flanker letters (somewhat similar to the paradigm used by Hübner & Lehle, 2007, and Lehle & Hübner, 2009), potentially leading to stronger compatibility effects than in the PP paradigm (Miller & Durst, 2015; Mittelstädt & Miller, 2017).

The key manipulation in both experiments was the S-R rule assigned to the flanker letters (i.e., Task 2), as is illustrated in Table 1. Specifically, we used three different flanker S-R rules and varied the rules between-subjects in order to avoid any possible confusions with having to change the S-R rule throughout the experiment. In the matched mapping condition, the S-R rule used for the flankers was identical to the S-R rule used for the center letter. For instance, if the letter “B” required a left key press when it appeared in the center, it also required a left key press when it appeared as a flanker. Consequently, in the matched condition, trials that were R2−R1 compatible were also S2−R1 compatible. For the example in Table 1, responses to the center target B in the stimulus CCBCC should be quite fast in the matched condition, because the flanker C is assigned to the same response as the target B within both S-R rules. Thus, the response would be facilitated both by S2−R1 activation and by R2−R1 activation. In the reversed mapping condition, the S-R rule used for the flankers was reversed relative to the S-R rule for the center letter. For example, if the letter “B” required a left key press in the center location, it would require a right key press appearing as a flanker. Thus, in the reversed condition, trials which are S2−R1 compatible are automatically R2−R1 incompatible. For the example in Table 1, responses to the center target B in the stimulus CCBCC should not be particularly fast in the reversed condition, because the flanker C is assigned to the opposite response within the T2 S-R rule. Thus, the response would be facilitated by S2−R1 activation but inhibited by any R2−R1 activation that is present. The third condition was the neutral mapping condition. Here, two distinct letter sets were used for the flankers and the center letters, and the two sets thus had separate S-R rules. Consequently, there were no cases of S2−R1 compatible trials in the neutral condition, and R2−R1 compatibility was the only possible source of compatibility effects. The neutral condition mainly served as a control condition with only R2−R1 compatibility, without S2−R1 compatibility.

Table 1 Exemplary stimulus-response rules and associated compatibility types for the flanker dual task

Figure 1 visualizes predictions for this experiment for the extreme cases in which BCEs are driven exclusively by S2−R1 compatibility (A and B) or exclusively by R2−R1 compatibility (C and D). Panels A and C plot the possible results in terms of S2−R1 compatibility; thus, in the example of Table 1, CCBCC would be classified as S2−R1 compatible and XXBXX would be classified as S2−R1 incompatible for both the matched and the reversed conditions. This is because—with compatibility defined in terms of the T1 S-R rule—the flankers C and X are associated with the matching and mismatching R1’s, respectively. Thus, if BCEs are driven exclusively by S2−R1 compatibility (A), responses should be faster in the S2−R1 compatible condition than in the S2−R1 incompatible condition, and this compatibility effect should be the same for both the matched and the reversed conditions (A). On the other hand, as is shown in Panel C, the pattern should look quite different in the reversed condition if BCEs are driven exclusively by R2−R1 compatibility. With this type of compatibility and the reversed T2 mapping condition, the response to the B in XXBXX would be relatively fast because the T2 S-R rule associates X with the same response that is required in T1 for B. Furthermore, the response to the B in CCBCC would be relatively slow because the T2 S-R rule associates C with the opposite response. Thus, if the BCE is driven by R2−R1 compatibility, the S2−R1 compatibility effect should invert in the reversed condition (C). Panels B and D re-plot the same results with trials classified in terms of R2−R1 compatibility, which changes the compatible/incompatible classification of trials in the reversed condition. Thus, in the example of Table 1, XXBXX would be classified as R2−R1 compatible because X is associated with the correct R1 within the T2 S-R rule. Similarly, CCBCC would now be classified as incompatible because C is associated with the incorrect R1 within the T2 S-R rule. With compatibility classified in this way, there should be a positive compatibility effect if the BCE is driven by R2−R1 compatibility (D), but a negative compatibility effect if the BCE is driven by S2−R1 compatibility (B). Note that there is no such thing as S2−R1 compatibility in the neutral condition, because the flanker letters in this condition are not associated with responses within the T1 S-R rule, so the neutral condition cannot be plotted in (A) and (C). There is still R2−R1 compatibility in the neutral condition, however, since the flankers are associated with responses in the T2 S-R rule. There would be no R2−R1 compatibility effect in this condition if the BCE is entirely stimulus-driven (B). If the BCE is entirely driven by R2−R1 compatibility, however, there would be the same positive BCE in the neutral condition as in the other two conditions (D).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Theoretical predictions of two models regarding the source of the BCE. The predictions of both models are displayed twice in the present figure, once as a function of stimulus compatibility (S2−R1; A and C), and once as a function of response compatibility (R2−R1; B and D). A and B: predictions if the BCE is purely stimulus-based. C and D: predictions if the BCE is purely response-based. See main text for explanation of the predictions. Because there was no S2−R1 compatibility in the neutral condition (see Table 1), this condition is not included in panels A and C. RT: reaction time, S: stimulus, R: response, BCE: backward crosstalk effect

In summary, the present experiments therefore allow us to disentangle stimulus (S2−R1) and response (R2−R1) compatibility, with S2−R1 compatibility referring to the idea that applying the T1 rule to S2 tends to activate R1 according to that rule. If the flankers directly activate R2 in time, the S2−R1 compatibility effect should be smaller in the reversed condition than in the matched condition—or possibly even reversed (i.e., a negative S2−R1 compatibility effect and a positive R2−R1 compatibility effect in the reversed condition). Conversely, if the BCE is mainly stimulus driven (i.e., only one S-R rule is active at a time), one should find no differences in the size of the S2−R1 compatibility effects between the matched and reversed conditions—and, consequently, a negative R2−R1 compatibility effect in the reversed condition. The neutral condition mainly served as a control condition with only R2−R1 compatibility, without S2−R1 compatibility.

Experiment 1

As mentioned above, in Experiment 1, the flanker task was embedded in a PP paradigm (Miller & Durst, 2014). In the present adaptation of the PP paradigm, participants are asked to first respond to the center letter, and only respond to the flanker letter if the center letter requires a no-go response. Consequently, any trial that requires a response to the center letter, ends after that response is executed, resembling the classic flanker task with just one overt response per trial. The PP paradigm shares many observable commonalities with the PRP paradigm (Miller & Durst, 2015), and also BCEs have been found in the PP paradigm (e.g., Miller & Durst, 2015; Mittelstädt & Miller, 2017). This therefore allows us to use this paradigm to investigate the BCEs that are typically present in dual-task situations, and try to further locate the sources of this BCE.

One previous study which investigated the BCE in the PP paradigm in more detail was Miller (2017). In this study, lateralized readiness potentials were used to determine the time at which BCEs arise during processing. The results suggested that BCEs are present because T2 stimuli influence T1 response selection (i.e., S2−R1 compatibility effect)—and not because T2 stimuli activate their corresponding R2 (i.e., no evidence for R2−R1 compatibility). The present experiment, then, aims at further shedding light on the nature of BCEs using a dual-task flanker paradigm, and at separating these two kinds of compatibility effects. Specifically, as mentioned above, we used three mapping conditions (i.e., matched: S2−R1 compatible trials were also R2−R1 compatible; reversed: S2−R1 compatible trials were R2−R1 incompatible; and neutral: S2 neutral to R1, and separate R2−R1 compatibility) to separate the contributions S2−R1 and R2−R1 compatibility.

Method

Participants

Participants were 60 University of Otago psychology students (46 women) who took part in the experiment in exchange for course credit. They ranged in age from 17 to 26 (M= 19.8) and they were predominantly right-handed (M = 59.6) as indexed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). We planned on sample sizes of 20 participants with accuracy above 80% per S-R mapping condition but actually tested one extra participant in the matched and neutral conditions due to the unpredictabilities associated with experimental participation. To obtain equal sample sizes in each condition, we dropped the participant with the lowest accuracy from the matched and neutral groups. We further excluded six additional participants due to low accuracy (i.e., below 80%).

Apparatus and stimuli

The experiment took place in individual test rooms. Stimulus presentation and recording of responses were controlled by an IBM-PC compatible computer using MATLAB with the Psychophysics Toolbox extension (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). Viewing distance was approximately 60 cm but not restrained. Stimuli were presented vertically and horizontally centered on a 17” screen in a white 35 point font—that is, a center letter was displayed flanked by two outside letters on each side. The letter stimuli were presented in white 35pt font. A centered, white plus sign (+) served as fixation point. Responses were key presses with the left and right index fingers on the “Z” and “?/” keys of a standard computer keyboard.

For each participant the letter stimuli were randomly selected from all consonants, excluding the letters L, R, and Z in order to avoid associations with response side or key. More specifically, two letters each were assigned for any of the three T1 response possibilities (i.e., left/right keypress, no-go). In the matched and reversed conditions, the same letters were used for T2 as for the go-stimuli for T1, with the same or reversed S-R rule, respectively. The T1 no-go letters never appeared as flankers in any condition. In the neutral condition, two additional stimuli were assigned to each possible response (i.e., left/right keypress) for T2, leading to a total of ten different letters used in this condition, and six different letters in the matched and reversed conditions. We only tested trials with different letters for the center and flanker stimuli, at least one of which was assigned to a go response, thus leaving 20 different trial types (we omitted one response-compatible stimulus combination in the neutral condition in order to obtain the same number of trial types in this condition).

Procedure

The single experimental session lasted approximately 45 min. Each subject was tested in one of the three conditions (i.e., matched, reversed, or neutral). The experiment consisted of two practice blocks and eight experimental blocks. The two practice blocks served as single-task training for T1 and T2, respectively, displaying the typical trial sequences but instructing participants to pay attention to only one of the two presented stimuli. In the experimental blocks, subjects were instructed to treat the center letter as the high priority task, and the flanker letters as the low priority task. That is, subjects were instructed to first respond to T1 with left/right index finger presses, respectively, and to only respond to T2 if S1 was the no-go stimulus. In the matched condition, the S-R rule was the same for T2 as for T1. In the reversed condition, the S-R rule for T2 was reversed to the rule for T1—that is, the stimuli which required a left response for T1 required a right response for T2 and vice versa. In the neutral condition, four additional letters served as the stimuli for T2 with two letters each corresponding to left/right responses, respectively. The mapping is illustrated in Table 1.

Each trial type was presented twice in the practice blocks (40 trials). The experimental blocks included 72 trials. In two-thirds of these trials, T1 required a response, and in one-third, T2 required a response. Of the T1 trials, half of the trials were R2−R1 compatible, and the other half were R2−R1 incompatible. In the trials where T1 required no response, half of the responses to T2 required a left-hand response and half required a right-hand response.

The trial sequence started with a fixation cross for 500 ms. The center letter and the flankers were displayed with an SOA of 100 ms. Stimuli remained on screen for a maximum of 2 s or until a response was given. Erroneous responses resulted in an error feedback screen display of 3 seconds. After responses faster than 200 ms or slower than 2 s, subjects were informed that they had responded too fast or too slow, respectively, for 3 s. If subjects made more than three consecutive errors, they were shown the instructions again as a reminder. Trials were followed by an intertrial interval of 2 s.

Results

Practice blocks and the first experimental block were excluded from any further analysis as training. Erroneous trials were removed from the RT analyses (8.7%). One and 34 trials were removed from the analyses as RT outliers based on lower and upper RT cutoffs of 200 ms and 2 s, respectively. The analyses comparing the matched and reversed conditions focus on stimulus compatibility. In the neutral mapping condition, there is no S2−R1 compatibility as the flanker letters are always drawn from a different letter set than the center letters—thus, for this condition, we refer to the R2−R1 compatibility.

Primary task: RT1 and PE1

Matched vs. reversed conditions

Figure 2A shows the means of RT1 for the matched and reversed mapping conditions as a function of S2−R1 compatibility. We ran an ANOVA with the between-subject factor condition (i.e., matched, reversed) and the within-subject factor of stimulus (i.e., flanker) compatibility for RT1. This ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of stimulus compatibility, F(1,38) = 51.119, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.574\), with faster responses in stimulus compatible (721 ms) than in stimulus incompatible (765 ms) trials. Interestingly, no other effect was significant (p s >.241), indicating no differences in compatibility effects between the matched and reversed conditions.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Experiment 1 results for the primary task as a function of both stimulus (i.e., S2−R1) compatibility (A, C, for RTs and percent errors) and response (i.e., R2−R1) compatibility (B, D, for RTs and percent errors), separately for each mapping condition. Note that as in the neutral condition, there was no S2−R1 compatibility and this condition is thus only present in the response compatibility plots. Please note that the figures on the right (B,D) replot the data from the figures on the left (A,C) referring to a different type of compatibility. Error bar represents the pooled standard error. RT: reaction time, PE: percent errors, S: stimulus, R: response

We ran parallel analyses on the percentage of erroneous responses for the primary task (PE1). Figure 2B visualizes the results of this ANOVA. This ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of stimulus compatibility, F(1,38) = 45.690, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.546\), with fewer erroneous responses in the stimulus compatible (4.3%) than in the stimulus incompatible (8.3%) trials. Interestingly, the condition x stimulus compatibility interaction was significant, \(F(1,38) = 15.972, p < .001, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.296\), indicating a larger stimulus compatibility effect in the matched (6.5%) than in the reversed (1.7%) condition, with the latter effect still significant, \(F(1,19) = 7.175, p=.015, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.274\). The main effect of condition was not significant, p =.519.

However, the interpretation of the PE data is not as straightforward in the PP paradigm, because R1 and R2 were both performed with the same response keys. It is therefore not possible to distinguish which of the two tasks the participant actually aimed to respond to, and there are most likely some trials where a participant incorrectly responded to T2 where s/he should have responded to T1. In the matched condition, the responses in these trials were correct in the S2−R1 compatible trials and incorrect in the reversed condition. Thus, these “wrong task” trials increase the S2−R1 BCE on PE in the matched condition. In the reversed condition, however, the opposite happens, and the “wrong task” trials decrease the compatibility effect. It might therefore be possible that this task confusion effect underlies the significant interaction reported above.

We checked whether the task (i.e., T1 vs. T2) responded to on trial n-1 played a role here, because it is reasonable to assume that this task confusion happens more often following a background task response. We thus added the task on trial n-1 as an additional factor in the ANOVA reported above. Figure 3 visualizes the separate ANOVAs for each mapping condition when including the n-1 task factor. This ANOVA produced a significant three-way interaction of condition, stimulus compatibility, and n-1 task, \(F(1,38) = 14.096, p=.001, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.271\). We followed this up by separately running ANOVAs for each mapping condition. In the matched condition, there was a significant interaction between n-1 task and stimulus compatibility, \(F(1,19) = 39.463, p < .001, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.675\). In the reversed condition, this interaction was not significant, \(F(1,19) = 0.955, p=.341, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.048\). As can be seen in Fig. 3A and B, it seems as if the larger S2−R1 compatibility effect just stems from the trials following a background task response. Thus, it seems plausible that this task confusion was responsible for the difference in the sizes of the S2−R1 compatibility effects between the matched and reversed conditions.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Percent errors (PE) as a function of S2−R1 compatibility (A and B for the matched and reversed conditions, respectively) and as a function of R2−R1 compatibility (C for the neutral condition), separately for trials following a trial where the previous (n-1) trial required a background task response or a primary task response. Error bar represents the pooled standard error. S: stimulus, R: response

Neutral condition

Figure 2C shows the means of RT1 for all groups (i.e., matched, reversed, neutral) as a function of R2−R1 compatibility, thus also showing the means of the neutral condition. In the neutral condition, we did not observe any R2−R1 compatibility effect, \(F(1,19) < 0.01, p=.939, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0\), with no difference between R2−R1 compatible (732 ms) and incompatible (732 ms) trials. Separate ANOVAs with each pair of conditions showed that this null effect was significantly different from the compatibility effects obtained in both the matched condition, \(F(1,38) = 20.275, p < .001, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.348\), and the reversed condition, \(F(1,38) = 10.486, p=.002, \eta ^{2}_{p}=0.216\).

We again ran parallel analyses on PE1, and Fig. 2D shows the means of PE1 for all three conditions as a function of R2−R1 compatibility. In the ANOVA of the neutral condition, responses in R2−R1 compatible trials were significantly less erroneous (4.7%) than in R2−R1 incompatible trials (8.0%), F(1,19) = 18.875, p < .001, \({\eta }^{2}_{p}=0.498\). Again, in the ANOVA comparing the neutral and matched conditions, there was a significant interaction of R2−R1 compatibility and condition (i.e., neutral vs. matched), F(1,38) = 6.315, p = .016, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.142\), indicating a smaller compatibility effect in the neutral (3.3%) than in the matched (6.5%) condition. In the ANOVA comparing the neutral and reversed conditions, the interaction of compatibility and mapping condition was not significant, F(1,38) = 2.705, p = .108, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.066\).

As was mentioned above, it is not possible to clearly distinguish which task a participant intended to respond to in the PP paradigm—possibly allowing task confusion effects in the error data to produce effects which look like compatibility effects. We therefore ran a parallel analysis including the n-1 task (i.e., T1 vs. T2) as an additional factor to the ANOVA reported above. The corresponding means of this ANOVA are displayed in Fig. 3C. There was a significant interaction of n-1 task and R2−R1 compatibility in the neutral condition, F(1,19) = 19.369, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.505\), indicating that the R2−R1 compatibility effect for PE reported above is mainly due to trials following a background task response. Because there was no R2−R1 compatibility effect in the RT data, we believe that this task confusion effect is the most likely explanation for the R2−R1 compatibility effect in the PE data.

Background task: RT2 and PE2

An ANOVA with the between-subject factor mapping condition was run for RT2. We found a significant main effect of condition, F(2,57) = 16.422, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.366\), with RT2 being fastest in the matched condition (828 ms), followed by the neutral condition (956 ms), and the slowest T2 responses in the reversed condition (1087 ms). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences between all three conditions (p s <.014). Note that it is not possible to examine effects on R2 of the match between R1 and R2 (i.e., forward compatibility) in the PP paradigm, because any trial with an R2 in the PP paradigm necessarily followed a no-go S1. Such effects will be examined in Experiment 2 using the PRP paradigm, however.

A parallel analysis was carried out for the percentage of erroneous responses for the background task (PE2). For PE2, a similar result pattern emerged. That is, the ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of condition, F(2,57) = 8.214, p = .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.224\). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a significant effect between the matched condition (9.9%) and the reversed condition (18.1%), p < .001, and between the neutral (12.6%) and the reversed condition, p = .015. The comparison between the matched and the neutral condition was not significant, p = .151.

Discussion

The main findings of Experiment 1 can be summarized as follows. In both the matched and the reversed conditions, we found a significant S2−R1 compatibility effect, with little change in this effect across the two mapping conditions, replicating the general existence of BCEs in the PP paradigm (e.g., Miller, 2017; Miller and Durst, 2015; Mittelstädt & Miller, 2017). In the neutral condition—which featured only R2−R1 compatibility—we found no compatibility effect.Footnote 1 Even though there was some evidence for an R2−R1 compatibility effect in the error data, we believe that this is mostly due to task confusion effects. This argument seems particularly plausible when considering that these R2−R1 compatibility effects stem only from trials that followed trials requiring a background task response—and it seems logical that this kind of task confusion (i.e., responding to T2 instead of T1) happens more often after responding to the background task.

The finding of a stimulus-based BCE on RT aligns well with previous findings in the PP paradigm. That is, it seems that the BCE is mainly based on S2−R1 compatibility, and not R2−R1 compatibility, aligning well with the findings of Miller (2017). S2−R1 compatibility seems to be the main source of the BCE in Experiment 1, as (a) the S2−R1 compatibility effect was also observed in the reversed condition, where S2−R1 compatible trials were R2−R1 incompatible, (b) this S2−R1 compatibility effect was not significantly decreased in the reversed compared to the matched condition, and (c) we did not observe any compatibility effect in the neutral condition, which only featured R2−R1 compatibility, but not S2−R1 compatibility. Moreover, there was also a main effect of mapping condition on RT2. Obviously, one would expect this effect because of mapping complications in the reversed and neutral conditions compared to the matched condition—however, our main concerns were regarding the BCEs in the different conditions.

However, as the PP paradigm only requires one overt response per trial, it is not clear whether that might have played a role in finding no evidence for an R2−R1 compatibility effect (or at least a decreased S2−R1 compatibility effect) in the reversed condition. Moreover, we also did not observe any R2−R1 compatibility effect in the neutral condition (which featured only R2−R1 but not S2−R1 compatibility), and this could also possibly have been due to the nature of the PP paradigm, where only one overt response per trial is required, with strong prioritization of T1. Therefore, Experiment 2 used a PRP paradigm—where on every trial two overt responses are necessary.

Experiment 2

Previous studies comparing the PRP and the PP paradigms have found larger BCEs in the former paradigm (Miller and Durst, 2015; Mittelstädt & Miller, 2017). This finding makes sense because in the PP paradigm, participants usually prioritize the first task more strongly than in the PRP paradigm, possibly leading to less interference by T2 characteristics. Moreover, Rieger, Mittelstädt, Dignath, and Kiesel (2019) suggested that in the PRP paradigm, motor coordination possibly decreases flexibility in T2 processing compared to the PP paradigm, thus potentially also allowing for more direct R2−R1 interference. As previous studies using both the PP and the PRP paradigm all used unrelated tasks (i.e., letter and color classification tasks), they did not distinguish between different types of compatibility (i.e., S2−R1 and R2−R1), as we are aiming to do in the present experiments. In Experiment 2, we thus embedded the flanker task in a PRP paradigm. Participants were asked to always first respond as fast and as accurately as possible to the center letter and subsequently to respond as fast and as accurately to the flankers. Experiment 2 thus largely mirrored Experiment 1—with the main difference of a higher priority of T2 in the PRP paradigm than in the PP paradigm.

BCEs in the PRP paradigm have been extensively researched (for a review, see for example, Lien & Proctor, 2002). However, most studies at least implicitly assumed that T2 interference is mainly based on R2 activation in time to influence T1, not directly differentiating between S2−R1 and R2−R1 compatibility effects. The present study aims at addressing this gap—and at investigating whether the lack of evidence for R2−R1 effects in the PP paradigm in the reversed condition is only due to the low priority of the second task (i.e., responding to the flankers). We used the same conditions as in Experiment 1, with the same S-R rules for center (T1) and flanker (T2) letters in the matched condition, with opposite S-R rules for the flankers in the reversed conditions, and with separate letter sets and S-R rules in the neutral condition.

Method

Participants

A fresh sample of 60 University of Otago psychology students (42 women, 15 men, three unknown) took part in the exchange for course credit.Footnote 2 Participants ranged in age from 18 to 26 (M = 19.7) and they were predominantly right-handed (M = 59.7), as indexed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). Additional six participants were also tested but excluded due to low accuracy (i.e., below 80%).

Apparatus, stimuli, procedure, and design

The apparatus, stimuli, procedure, and instructions were the same as in Experiment 1 except for the following changes. As mentioned above, the dual-task paradigm in which the flanker task was embedded was a PRP paradigm. Consequently, participants were asked to always first respond to the center letter and then subsequently respond to the outside letters. We omitted no-go stimuli from the experiment, as the PRP paradigm does not necessitate no-go-stimuli for either task, leaving four different letters in the matched and reversed conditions, and eight different letters in the neutral condition. Thus, participants had to give two responses on every trial. Omitting the no-go stimuli lead to fewer letters used for each participant. That is, in the matched and reversed conditions, four letters were used (i.e., both appearing as center and as flanker letters) for each participant; in the neutral condition, eight different letters (i.e., four as the center letters, four as the flanker letters) were used for each participant. Consequently, the number of trial types was also reduced to 12. In order to keep the experiment length similar to Experiment 1, each block had 64 trials. Each R2−R1 compatible trial type was presented eight times (as in Experiment 1, we excluded trials with the same stimulus for the center and the flankers) whereas each R2−R1 incompatible trial type was presented four times–resulting in 50% compatible and 50% incompatible trials.

Results

Exclusion criteria for the analyses were the same as in Experiment 1. That is, practice blocks and the first experimental block were excluded from any further analysis as training. Trials in which any error was made were removed from the RT analyses (10.28%). None and 140 trials were removed from the analyses as RT outliers based on lower and upper RT cutoffs of 200 ms and 2 s, respectively.

Task 1: RT1 and PE1

Matched vs. reversed conditions

Figure 4A shows the means of RT1 for the matched and reversed conditions as a function of S2−R1 compatibility. We ran an ANOVA with the between-subject factor condition (i.e., matched, reversed) and the within-subject factor of S2−R1 compatibility for RT1. The main effect of stimulus compatibility was again significant, F(1,38) = 189.29, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.833\), with faster responses in stimulus compatible (894 ms) than in stimulus incompatible (1087 ms) trials. Moreover, there was a main effect of mapping condition, F(1,38) = 19.374, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.338\), indicating faster responses in the matched condition (912 ms), than in the reversed condition (1068 ms). As in Experiment 1, the interaction of mapping condition and stimulus compatibility was not significant, F(1,38) = 1.412, p = .242, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.036\), indicating that the size of the stimulus compatibility effects was not modulated by condition (matched: 209 ms, reversed: 176 ms).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Experiment 2 results for the first task as a function of both stimulus (i.e., S2−R1) compatibility (A, C, for RTs and percent errors) and response (i.e., R2−R1) compatibility (B, D, for RTs and percent errors), separately for each condition. Please note that the figures on the right side (B,D) replot the data from the figures on the left (A,C) referring to a different type of compatibility. Error bar represents the pooled standard error. RT: reaction time, PE: percent errors, S: stimulus, R: response

We again conducted parallel analyses on the percentage of erroneous responses for the first task (PE1). Figure 4C shows the means of PE1 for the matched and reversed conditions as a function of S2−R1 compatibility. This ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of stimulus compatibility, F(1,38) = 52.851, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.582\), indicating fewer erroneous responses in stimulus compatible (4.3%) than in stimulus incompatible (8.3%) trials. The main effect of condition was significant, F(1,38) = 12.888, p = .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.253\), indicating fewer erroneous responses in the matched (7.5%) than in the reversed (12.5%) condition. The interaction of stimulus compatibility and mapping condition was not significant, F(1,38) = 1.850, p = .182, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.046\), indicating no differences in the stimulus compatibility effects between the matched (5.6%) and reversed (8.3%) conditions.

Neutral condition

Figure 4B shows the means of RT1 for all conditions (i.e., matched, reversed, neutral) as a function of R2−R1 compatibility, thus also showing the means of the neutral condition. Contrasting the findings of Experiment 1, we found a significant R2−R1 compatibility effect in the neutral condition, F(1,19) = 23.206, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.55\), with faster R2−R1 compatible (951 ms) than on R2−R1 incompatible (1083 ms) trials, resulting in a 132 ms R2−R1 compatibility effect. Comparing this compatibility effect to the one obtained in the matched condition, we found a larger effect in the matched (209 ms) than in the neutral (132 ms) condition, F(1,38) = 5.329, p = .027, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.123\). Moreover, the compatibility effect obtained in the neutral condition did not differ significantly from the effect obtained in the reversed condition, F(1,38) = 1.566, p = .218, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.04\).

We again ran parallel analyses on PE1, and Figure 4D shows the means of PE1 for all three conditions as a function of R2−R1 compatibility. Again, we found a significant effect of R2−R1 compatibility in the neutral condition, F(1,19) = 8.729, p = .008, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.315\), with fewer erroneous responses in compatible (6.9%) than in incompatible (11.5%) trials. This compatibility effect did not differ in size with the one obtained in the matched condition, F(1,38) = 1.285, p = .264, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.033\). Comparing this effect to the compatibility effect obtained in the reversed condition, there was a significant interaction, F(1,38) = 5.995, p = .019, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.136\), with a larger effect in the reversed (8.3%) than in the neutral (3.6%) condition.

Task 2: RT2 and PE2

In Experiment 2, we conducted similar analyses for T2 as for T1. That is, we ran an ANOVA with the between-subject factor condition (i.e., matched, reversed, neutral) and the within-subject factor forward compatibility (i.e., compatible, incompatible second task stimulus to the first task stimulus). We again refer to stimulus compatibility here. As mentioned above, there is no possibility of stimulus compatibility in the neutral condition, therefore we always refer to response compatibility in this condition, generally labeling compatibility regarding the second task forward compatibility.

The ANOVA revealed a significant effect of forward compatibility, F(1,57) = 171.06, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.75\), indicating shorter RT2s in the forward compatible trials (968 ms) than in the forward incompatible (1167 ms) trials. Moreover, we found a significant effect of condition, F(2,57) = 6.943, p = .002, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.196\). That is, RT2 was significantly shorter in the matched (974 ms) than in the reversed (1145 ms) condition (p<.001), as well as than in the neutral condition (1084 ms, p =.019). The difference between the reversed and the neutral condition was not significant (p=.267). The mapping condition x forward compatibility interaction was significant, F(2,57) = 3.673, p = .032, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.114\).

We conducted separate ANOVAs to further locate this interaction, always excluding one mapping condition at a time. The ANOVA including the matched and neutral conditions yielded a significant interaction, F(1,38) = 7.035, p = .012, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.156\), indicating a significantly larger forward compatibility effect in the matched (249 ms) than in the neutral condition (148 ms). The interactions of condition and forward compatibility when excluding the matched or neutral conditions were not significant (p =.177 and p=.180, respectively).

We ran a parallel analyses for the percentage of erroneous responses to the second task, PE2. This ANOVA revealed a significant effect of forward compatibility, F(1,57) = 59.467, p < .001, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.511\), indicating lower PE2 in forward compatible (7.4%) than forward incompatible (13.2%) trials. Moreover, the main effect of condition was significant, F(2,57) = 6.686, p = .002, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.19\). Pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences between the matched (7.5%) and the reversed (12.5%) conditions (p<.001), as well as between the matched and the neutral (10.9%) conditions (p = .015). The difference between the reversed and neutral conditions was not significant (p=.298). The interaction of forward compatibility and mapping condition just failed to be significant, F(2,57) = 3.099, p = .053, \(\eta ^{2}_{p}=0.098\).

Discussion

The results of Experiment 2 largely mirrored the findings of Experiment 1. That is, we found no interaction between the matched and reversed conditions’ S2−R1 compatibility effects, and the compatibility effect in the reversed condition was in the direction of an S2−R1 compatibility effect, and not based on an R2−R1 compatibility effect. The main difference to the findings of Experiment 1 is that we found evidence for a R2−R1 compatibility effect in the neutral condition. Thus, one cannot entirely discount the possibility that response compatibility does play a role as a BCE source—though evidently a lesser one than stimulus compatibility. We will return to this idea in the General Discussion. Moreover, the overall BCEs were descriptively much larger in the PRP paradigm (173 ms over all three mapping conditions) than in the PP paradigm (31 ms over all three mapping conditions), and this finding aligns well with the findings of Miller and Durst (2015) who also found smaller BCEs in the PP paradigm than in the PRP paradigm. In addition, Janczyk, Renas, and Durst (2018) also found that a task produces smaller BCEs when it does not always require a response, aligning well with the between-paradigm differences obtained in the present experiments. Whether or not a second response always has to be executed therefore seems to be important to the size of response compatibility-based BCEs.

Even though the overall compatibility effects were descriptively much larger, we still did not find evidence for direct R2 activation in the reversed condition. As in Experiment 1, in the reversed condition, the BCE was in the direction of an S2−R1 compatibility effect and did not differ in size from the S2−R1 compatibility effect found in the matched condition. It is interesting to note that in the reversed condition, R1 in a left-left response sequence was slower than in a left-right response sequence, and this must have been based on the S2−R1 compatibility in these trials. Because response grouping can possibly affect effects observed in first task RTs, we checked quite thoroughly whether the result pattern changed when requiring different minimum inter-response-intervals (IRIs) and found that response grouping did not affect the general result pattern.Footnote 3

General discussion

The present study aimed at disentangling stimulus and response compatibility as different sources of BCEs in dual-tasking paradigms using the same response sets for both tasks. To this end, we embedded the classic flanker task within two dual-task paradigms (i.e., the PP and PRP paradigms), assigning an S-R rule to the flanker letters that was either matched with respect to the center letter’s S-R rule (i.e., same S-R rule for the flankers as for the center letters), reversed (i.e., opposite S-R assignment for the flankers), or neutral (i.e., separate stimulus sets for the two tasks). Participants were always asked to respond first to the center letter (T1) and subsequently to the flanker letters in some trials (Experiment 1, responding only when the center letter required no response) or in all trials (Experiment 2).

The main results were rather consistent. Specifically, in both experiments, we found an S2−R1 compatibility effect in both the matched and the reversed conditions, with this effect changing little across the two mapping conditions, which suggests that R2−R1 compatibility had little effect. Thus, these results indicate that the BCE in these conditions was mainly driven by stimulus- rather than response-based compatibility. In the neutral condition, the results differed between the experiments: there was no R2−R1 compatibility effect for RTs in Experiment 1, and a 132 ms R2−R1 compatibility effect in Experiment 2. Thus, even though it might be a lesser role than the one played by stimulus compatibility, we cannot fully discount a role for response compatibility as a BCE source (see also Miller, 2006; Miller & Alderton, 2006; Ruiz Fernández & Ulrich, 2010).

Implications for response selection

Regarding response selection, the most straightforward interpretation of the results obtained in the matched and reversed conditions is that only one S-R rule produced activation at a time. Specifically, it seems likely that S2 directly activated the corresponding T1 response based on the T1 S-R rule and thus created facilitation or interference relative to that rule, without activating R2 based on the T2 S-R rule. For instance, consider Experiment 2, where two overt responses were necessary on every trial: Here, R1 in the reversed condition was slower in same-hand response sequences (e.g., left-left) than in opposite-hand response sequences (e.g., left-right). If S2 had directly activated the corresponding response based on the second-task S-R rule, one would expect faster R1’s with same-hand response sequences (i.e., R2−R1 compatible) than with opposite-hand response (i.e., R2−R1 incompatible) sequences, just as they were in the matched and neutral conditions. This was not the case, however, so these results suggest that the BCEs observable in RT1 were driven by stimulus compatibility (i.e., an association between S2 and R1) rather than by preliminary activation of R2 by S2.

The stimulus-based compatibility effects in the matched and reversed conditions appear to be compatible with RSB accounts of dual tasking, because it seems as if only one S-R rule was active at a time—therefore potentially allowing for sequential response selection stages in the two tasks. However, the compatibility effects in these conditions do not seem to be consistent with the automatic response activation account of BCEs (e.g., Hommel, 1998), which was an initial extension to the classic bottleneck account of dual-task performance (e.g., Pashler, 1994). Specifically, if S2 activated R2 in time to influence T1 performance in accordance with the automatic activation account, then we should have observed a significantly decreased S2−R1 compatibility effect in the reversed condition compared to the matched condition. In fact, if R2 activation by S2 were the main source of the BCE, then the S2−R1 compatibility effect could even have been flipped in the reversed condition, with the combination of incompatible S2 stimuli and compatible R2 responses producing faster T1 responses than the reverse combination. Obviously, this did not happen.

However, we also have to consider the results obtained in the neutral condition. Here, the results differed between the two experiments—that is, we only found evidence for an effect of R2−R1 compatibility in the dual-task setting of the latter experiment. Nevertheless, the R2−R1 compatibility effect in Experiment 2 clearly shows the possibility of T2 interference independent from S2−R1 compatibility, and this is generally in line with earlier findings demonstrating that T1-irrelevant characteristics of T2 can still influence T1 performance (e.g., Miller & Alderton, 2006; Ruiz Fernández & Ulrich, 2010). Like many earlier BCEs, the neutral-condition R2−R1 compatibility effect seems to contradict pure RSB accounts of dual-task performance and to require an extension involving something like automatic response activation (Hommel, 1998).

As was argued above, the combined findings of the matched and reversed conditions seem to be inconsistent with the automatic response activation account, however, because the BCEs in these conditions were not based on S2 activating R2, but on S2 activating the corresponding R1 based on the T1 S-R rule. Using an extended PRP approach with three tasks that were less related than the two tasks in our present research, Janczyk et al., (2018) also argued that BCEs are located at the central response selection stage of processing. This idea would be consistent with the present conclusions, because we also argue that the combined results of the matched and reversed conditions contradict the idea of a separate stage generating response activation associated with a later task, and the findings in the neutral condition of Experiment 2 seem to contradict pure RSB accounts with non-overlapping response selection stages of the two tasks.

How can we then reconcile the present findings in all three mapping conditions and both experiments? For one thing, it might be necessary for models that aim at explaining BCEs to distinguish between situations with the same or different stimuli for the two tasks. Specifically, in situations with the same stimuli for T1 and T2, we see two closely-related accounts for the apparent absence of R2-based activations in the reversed condition. One possibility is that just one of the conflicting S-R rules can be active in working memory at a time. For example, while the first task is being performed, the second-task S-R rule might be fully inhibited in order to suppress any possible conflict. Such inhibition would not be needed when the two tasks used different stimuli, because there is no direct conflict in that case. Clearly, with only one active rule, S2 would only produce activation according to that rule, so S2 would not activate R2 and R2−R1 compatibility would have no effect. The other, closely related, possibility is that both S-R rules are active but that any given stimulus can only activate a single response, and the response that it activates is the one prescribed by the single most active S-R rule which is applicable to that stimulus. Thus, while working on T1 in the reversed condition, S2 would only produce activation according to the T1 S-R rule because this rule would be most active. Again, there would be no S2−R2 activation and hence no R2−R1 compatibility effect.

It is interesting that there was a between-experiment discrepancy with respect to the existence of a R2−R1 BCE in the neutral condition. Specifically, we found no R2−R1 compatibility effect in the PP paradigm where participants only had to respond overtly to T2 in one third of all trials (Experiment 1), but we did find an R2-R1 compatibility effect in the PRP paradigm where participants had to produce an R2 on every trial (Experiment 2). This discrepancy is probably explained by the greater relative T2 importance in the PRP paradigm, where it more often requires a response and would therefore have a higher priority (Miller and Durst, 2015). Furthermore, regarding other between-paradigm differences, Rieger et al., (2019) suggested that the coordination of two motor responses plays a crucial role in prioritizing tasks, which was only necessary in the PRP but not in the PP paradigm. Moreover, the extent to which T2 is processed online is different between the two paradigms (Mittelstädt and Miller, 2017), with stronger interference from T2 to T1 in the PRP paradigm than in the PP paradigm. Summarizing, differences in the characteristics of the two paradigms seem to be responsible for the existence of the BCE in the neutral condition in the PRP paradigm and its absence in the PP paradigm.

Links to evidence from lateralized readiness potentials

Another way to study response activation in BCEs is to use lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs). Voluntary hand movements are preceded by an LRP (Deecke, Grözinger, & Kornhuber, 1976) which can be observed in electroencephalographic activity. This LRP is a reliable measure of hand-specific motor preparation (e.g., Osman, Moore, & Ulrich, 1995; Smulders & Miller, 2012). The onset of the LRP therefore provides a specific marker in time for the onset of hand-specific motor activation, and this marker can be used to subdivide the RT interval into subintervals before and after the onset of this motor activity (e.g., Hackley & Valle-Inclan, 1998; Miller & Ulrich, 1998). Experimental effects can thus be located to time intervals either before motor activation begins (i.e., stimulus-locked LRP effects), or after that point (i.e., response-locked LRP effects).

In LRP studies using the PRP paradigm (Ko & Miller, 2014) and the PP paradigm (Miller, 2017), both choice-related and no-go-related forms of the BCE seemed to be located prior to the onset of response activation (i.e., the BCE produces stimulus-locked LRP effects). Specifically, Ko and Miller (2014) addressed the question of whether the go/no-go selection-related processes of T2 influence RT1 before and/or after the onset of the LRP associated with R1. To this end, they used a letter classification task with manual response (i.e., left/right keypress) as T1 and a go/no-go foot press auditory task as T2. They found a no-go BCE which was located prior to T1 response initiation (i.e., stimulus-locked LRP effect). Similarly, Miller (2017) investigated the locus of the choice-related BCE in the PP paradigm. In this study, T1 was a letter classification task, T2 was a two-choice color classification task, and again the key finding was that the BCE affected the stimulus-locked LRP of T1. The LRP results of both studies thus suggest that BCEs arise because S2 influences T1 response selection rather than because it activates the corresponding R2 with which it is associated, much in line with our present findings.

Links to action effect based compatibility effects

The present evidence that stimulus compatibility is more critical to the BCE than response compatibility also fits well with research on action effect compatibility (e.g., Janczyk et al., 2014). According to ideomotor theory (see for example, Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001; Pfister & Janczyk, 2012; Stock & Stock, 2004), responses and other actions are cognitively coded in terms of their perceived effects, so they are faster when the action effects are compatible with the actions themselves (i.e., action-effect compatibility effects; e.g., Kunde, 2001 Kunde, Hoffmann, & Zellmann, 2002; Pfister, Kiesel, & Melcher, 2010). For example, left- and right-hand responses are faster when they produce lights appearing on the same side (i.e., left vs. right) than when they produce lights that appear on the opposite side.

Action-effect compatibility effects have also been observed in dual-task paradigms similar to those used in the present studies. For example, Janczyk et al., (2014) used a dual-task paradigm resembling a PRP task, and in different conditions R2 could lead to either a compatible (e.g., light appearing on the same side as the response) or incompatible (e.g., light appearing on the opposite side to the response) action effect. In blocks with incompatible action effects, they found that the standard R2−R1 BCE disappeared—and descriptively even reversed—suggesting that response compatibility was less important than action effect compatibility. As argued above, our findings indicate that response compatibility is also less important than stimulus compatibility. Together, these results raise the question of whether response compatibility is rather low in a hierarchy of potential BCE sources. Taking the action effect account to an extreme, in fact, one could even argue that any case of response compatibility is really only based on action effect compatibility, because any response in a typical multitasking paradigm also has at least the effect of generating small proprioceptive feedback from emitting that response on the keyboard. These kinds of effects obviously cannot be completely disentangled from making the actual response itself, making it difficult to be sure whether the BCE is driven by the activation of the response or the expected action effect.

Conclusion

We embedded the classic flanker task in two different dual-task paradigms to separate the contributions of second-task stimulus (S2−R1) and response (R2−R1) compatibility to BCEs. To this end, we varied the S-R rule between-subjects to be either matched, reversed, or neutral. The results across both experiments suggest that the BCE is mainly driven by stimulus compatibility (i.e., S2−R1) rather than by response compatibility (i.e., R2−R1), because the BCEs in both the matched and reversed conditions were in the direction of S2−R1 compatibility effects—with little change in this effect across mapping conditions. It thus seems that when the T1 S-R rule is applicable to S2, this rule (and consequently, the resulting stimulus compatibility) is what drives the BCE, rather than R2 activation generated by applying the T2 S-R rule to S2, contrary to some previous theoretical accounts of the BCE. This stimulus compatibility-based BCE seems generally consistent with RSB models of dual tasking, and seems generally incompatible with the extension of automatic response activation. However, the results obtained in the second experiment’s neutral condition (i.e., evidence for R2−R1 compatibility) would need such an extension, and it might be therefore necessary to distinguish situations with the same or different stimuli for the two tasks.