“A theory is a generative framework that not only enhances our understanding of phenomena, but also yields predictive principles that can anticipate solutions to new problems and novel applications” [1]

Introduction

“Ok everyone, we’re here to do a sim, this is a safe space, nothing that is said or done here will leave the room”. “You’ve all done the work, you’ll all be fine. Just go in there and I’m sure you’ll be fabulous”. “This is a safe place to make mistakes—no patients will be harmed”. There is little doubt that people who have navigated themselves to this paper will have either said or heard these words in the context of health simulation. These words are quite comforting to say, and we genuinely want them to be true. They form part of a script that relies on adages for which we have become accustomed: simulation provides a psychologically safe space to rehearse skills, to make mistakes and to avoid patient harm. But just because these statements can be true does not mean that they always are true.

This is by no means the first paper that has challenged some of the conventions, myths and practices that have been enthusiastically adopted in health simulation practice and research, and likely won’t be the last. Further to critiquing the problems or the debate about the problems that exist in simulation practice, this paper seeks to explore the principles and practice of simulation through a different lens. The lens we will look through may generate deeper consideration of why some approaches to working with participants in simulations and simulation programmes work better than others (consider in situ vs non-in situ simulations, variation in approach of debriefers, longitudinal debriefing, perceptions of psychological safety) and how we can improve and optimise our simulation learning environments.

Just as the practice of designing and delivering simulation has been evolving to meet the needs of learners and institutions, so too has the research in this field. As with all other previously emergent fields of research, there is an imperative to (a) reflect on how the quality and direction of research endeavours can be strengthened and (b) act on recommendations that will allow the field to fulfil its potential. In their 2022 editorial, Walter Eppich and Gabriel Reedy note the general change of direction in health simulation research as moving away from aims that seek to justify simulation activities to those which seek to clarify how, and in which circumstances, simulation is effective [2]. Their call to action is clear and framed by three guiding principles:

  1. 1.

    Theoretical frameworks and concepts must be better integrated into all phases of the design and execution of research projects and programmes of research.

  2. 2.

    Varied methodologies and methodological lenses are required to progress the field.

  3. 3.

    Innovative techniques for data collection and analysis should be explored and embraced.

We have been challenged, as a simulation research community, to more deeply consider theory, methodology and methods as they relate to health simulation and health simulation research [2].

The theory that is the focus of this paper is self-determination theory (SDT). SDT focuses on human motivation and behaviours and has informed the growth of various fields [3, 4]. Whilst referred to in some health simulation literature [5,6,7], it has yet to be comprehensively applied, explored or tested in this field. This paper forms a foundation for discussing some promising lines of research enquiry that could help advance the field of health simulation. It offers an overview of a theoretical framework that appears to be both relevant to health simulation and that offers a variety of methodologies to explore simulation for new insights and areas for practice improvement.

The theory

Self-determination theory (SDT) is described as a macro theory (in this instance, an overarching theory) of human motivation [1]. First proposed in the 1970s by Richard Ryan and Edward Deci, it has been broadly applied, explored and tested in numerous settings and populations, including primary and secondary schools [8, 9], universities [10,11,12], workplaces and various health contexts [13].

The origins of SDT lie in the exploration of human motivation and the conditions and environments that impact human behaviours [14]. Over the past four decades, it has slowly and organically developed into a broader theory, which now includes six related “mini-theories” [14]. The mini-theories of SDT include the following: cognitive evaluation theory, organismic integration theory, Causality Orientations Theory, Basic Needs Theory, Goal Content Theory and the Relationships Motivation Theory [15,16,17] (see Table 1). These interrelated theories offer numerous opportunities for considering foundational principles that may already, and perhaps ought to, underpin the design and delivery of health simulation activities and programmes.

Table 1 Overview of the six mini-theories of self-determination theory (SDT)

One of the early propositions in SDT was that the motivations that lead to behaviours (or inaction) could be separated into categories: those that are self-determined (“i.e. governed by the process of choice and experienced as emanating from the self”) and those that are initiated or determined by factors external to the self (“i.e. governed by the process of compliance and experienced as compelled by some interpersonal or intrapsychic force”) [19]. These have come to be known as intrinsic and extrinsic motivational forces.

In SDT, the identified types of motivation are often visualised on a spectrum. At the upper end of this spectrum lies “intrinsic motivation” — behaviours that emanate from a sense of self and that are inherently satisfying [15]. This is followed by four states of “extrinsic motivation”: external regulation, introjection, identification and integration [1]. Finally, at the lower end of the spectrum lies “amotivation” — a state where an individual lacks any intention to act [1, 13].

Intrinsic motivation is explored in the first mini-theory of SDT: cognitive evaluation theory — a theory that is concerned with the factors that either undermine or support intrinsic motivation [15]. Intrinsic motivation is described in this theory as a type of self-determined motivation. It is a construct that “describes [a] natural inclination toward assimilation, mastery, spontaneous interest, and exploration” [20]. It has long been acknowledged as developing in humans from birth and, operationally, describes behaviour adopted for its inherently satisfying results [15]. Notably, enjoyment that stems from intrinsic motivation is likely to be conducive to personal growth and eudaimonia — a state of living a “complete” life or “living life well” [21]. Also to note, experiments undertaken in the pursuit of exploring this theory have found that some types of rewards can decrease people’s intrinsic motivation [3].

The aim of all behaviour is most unlikely to be all intrinsically motivated — there are countless internal and external pressures that prompt various behaviours [22]. Whilst intrinsically motivated behaviours are a significant type of self-determined behaviours, they are not the only form of self-determined behaviours. There are numerous extrinsically motivated behaviours that are also said to be self-determined. These are further explored in organismic integration theory, which posits that distinct characteristics of various extrinsically motivated behaviours can be identified [18].

Extrinsically motivated behaviours are those that are undertaken to obtain an external outcome (for example wealth, notoriety or material goods) [23]. In SDT, the study of extrinsic motivation has been much more concerned with the quality of motivation, as opposed to the quantity of motivation [23, 24]. This is in contrast to other theories of motivation, particularly as they relate to employment, which often focus on the quantity of motivation that individuals possess in relation to particular tasks [23]. Opportunities to consider this spectrum in health simulation are explored below and culminate in some hypothesised example statements in Table 2.

Table 2 Examples of motivational construct statements

Ordered from the least to the most internalised of the four subcategories of extrinsic motivation are as follows: external regulation, introjection, identification and integration (see descriptions in Table 2). These lie on a continuum of self-determination and, when exercised, produce demonstrably different outcomes and associated outputs. External regulation and introjected motivation are forms of “non-self-determined” motivation [15, 23]. Behaviours that fall into the category of extrinsic motivation are regulated by an external pressure or an external reward, such as financial remuneration or threat of punishment [13, 25]. Identification and integration are considered to be autonomous and self-regulated forms of extrinsic motivation [15, 25].

Identifying that there are qualitative differences that underlie peoples’ extrinsically motivated behaviours is important [23] (consider your experiences of working with simulation participants who love simulation, versus those who attend because it is a requirement of their job or education). Evaluating these differences holds value for understanding human behaviour, and how our social environment and work systems can be designed to optimise human potential [1].

Amotivation sits at the opposite end of the spectrum from intrinsic motivation. When amotivation is experienced in a workplace, for example, an employee may value an activity or behaviour so little that no effort is exerted to complete or realise the potential of that behaviour [13] (for a health-related example, consider the issues of poor adherence to appropriate hand hygiene).

To illustrate the different constructs of motivation, example statements relating to the qualities of motivation to participating in physical exercise, as presented by Ng et al. [13], are provided in Table 2. Alongside, these sit some potential statements relating to health professionals, and the quality of motivation to gain consent from patients, and examples relating to participating in simulation activities.

SDT is concerned not only with the quality of motivation but also the types of environments and contexts that effect motivation and the changes in motivation people may experience. organismic integration theory asserts that people are inherently driven towards learning, mastery and connection [16]. This inherent quality, however, is not achieved without conditions that are supportive. These conditions are believed to include three fundamental psychological needs: autonomy, competence and relatedness [24]. Indeed, the presence or absence of conditions that support these basic needs may “sustain [or] diminish the “innate propensity” of humans to act from an intrinsic motivation” [20]. The examination of intrinsic motivation has therefore been one that has evaluated these conditions and forms the basis for basic psychological needs theory.

Three basic psychological needs are explored in basic psychological needs theory: autonomy, competence and relatedness. In the context of SDT, autonomy refers to the “the perception of being the origin of one’s own behavior and experiencing volition in action” [13], and is not defined by autonomy’s other definitions which relate to independence and separation [17].

Autonomy has been explored at length in terms of both the individual experience and the contexts that either support or inhibit this psychological need [16]. Autonomy supportive environments include those that encourage and allow individuals to experience their behaviour as volitional. Features of autonomy supportive environments include nonjudgemental attitudes, the provision of rationales for suggestions or decisions and the facilitation of self-regulation [26].

Competence is described as “the feeling of being effective in producing desired outcomes and exercising one’s capacities” [13]. It is concerned with mastery [16]. It has been identified that “the need for competence is best satisfied within well-structured environments that afford optimal challenges, positive feedback and opportunities for growth” [16]. It is not hard to draw links between this statement and the practice of health simulation. We can hypothesise that the high levels of satisfaction students report when participating in simulation event(s) are inextricably linked to the efforts made to create a structured environment and to provide feedback through debriefing that is both positive and directive for growth.

Relatedness is defined in SDT as the “feeling of being respected, understood, and cared for by others” [13]. Relationship motivation theory is the newest of the six mini-theories and focuses on the impact of basic psychological needs on interpersonal relationships. A central idea in Relationship Motivational Theory is mutuality of autonomy [3]. In other words, the equal creation of autonomy-supportive environments from each party. This idea has interesting implications for the relationship that develops between facilitators and participants and indeed between participants themselves.

How has SDT already been applied to simulation?

A handful of studies have been published that investigate links between elements of SDT and the design of health simulation scenarios, activities and programmes. Table 3 provides a brief overview of the studies which have identified SDT itself or elements of SDT in their study. They include three prospective, quantitative studies [25, 27, 28]; two mixed-methods studies [29, 30] and one qualitative study [5]. SDT was also mentioned in a discussion paper regarding mastery learning, but not extensively explored [6].

Table 3 Examples of SDT in current simulation and medical education literature

As can be seen in Table 3, the aims and hypotheses being explored are somewhat varied, but all have a focus on motivation. For example, in the studies conducted by Diaz-Agea, Pujalte-Jesus [5] and Escher and Rystedt [30], motivation to participate in the simulations themselves is explored in cohorts of nursing students and health professionals respectively. In the Henry and Vesel [29] example, motivation was explored in relation to participants’ feedback-seeking behaviours. Autonomy is the other SDT element that is explored, with studies working to determine its relationship with different types of motivation [25, 28].

Two questionnaires that have been developed in the exploration of SDT were used in the studies included in Table 3: The inventory of intrinsic motivation (IMI) scale and the Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS). The IMI derives from one of the mini-theories of SDT: cognitive evaluation theory [31]. There are numerous versions of this questionnaire which have been adapted for different contexts (e.g. sport, physical education) and experiments which have tested cognitive evaluation theory [28, 31]. The SIMS is a validated tool that invites participants to respond to prompts linked to four types of motivation: intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, external regulation and amotivation [32].

Beyond the discrete cases of SDT being investigated in health simulation in the examples provided, there is no current programme of research that is exploring this theory in relation to health simulation. A broader and deeper exploration of the theory and its relevance to health simulation is warranted. It is warranted because of the following: (1) there is a necessity for our field to better understand theoretical foundations that may facilitate progress, and appropriate reform, in the design and delivery of simulation, (2) there is a growing demand for theory to underpin our own professional development as simulationists [2, 33], (3) there is potential for this deeper understanding of practice to enhance outcomes for learners and patients and (4) there are existing parallels between the language used in the study of SDT and the practice of health simulation.

Current and future implications

We have opportunities to more deeply consider the fundamental principles that underpin health simulation and to determine what elements of SDT could lead to improvements in the design and delivery of health simulation activities, programmes and research.

The conceptual argument for this is founded in some assumptions. Namely, that SDT (1) is a relevant theory to consider when exploring how and why simulation is an effective modality for technical and behavioural skill development in the health simulation context, (2) offers new avenues for exploring how simulations can be designed with enhanced and predictable participant benefit, (3) may be relevant in explaining why people who deliver simulation activities (including simulated patients, embedded participants and simulation coordinators) value participating in this type of activity and (4) has the potential to explain why simulation is a successful modality for learning, skill development, team building and for improving system functionality and safety.

At face value, it does appear that the principles that have guided health simulation activities can be firmly linked to foundational components of SDT. If we consider the often adopted “basic assumption”, through the lens of SDT, we can see alignment between language and theory: (“We believe that everyone participating in this simulation is intelligent, capable [competence], cares about doing their best [autonomy, competence, motivation] and wants to improve [motivation]”) [34].

In moving from an intuited to an explicit practice of psychological safety that is founded in SDT, we can apply evidence from the broader health professions and clinical education literature. This literature strongly suggests psychological safety can be provided and optimised when an “autonomy supportive” environment is created and sustained [11, 35]. The benefits of autonomy supportive environments include the increased intrinsic motivation of learners (i.e. learners experience deep satisfaction in the learning process and are intrinsically motivated to continue that learning process). Examples of how the features of autonomy supportive environments may already, or could, be applied to health simulation are outlined in Table 4.

Table 4 Example features of autonomy-supportive environments, as applied to health simulation

In efforts to understand the foundations of good quality health simulation, and to further explore the validity of the various components of SDT in this field, research projects can address quite a broad array of questions. It would be relevant to examine how SDT could further inform simulation design and delivery (as described above), simulation participants’ quality of motivation to transfer technical and behavioural skills to the clinical environment, how principles and evidence from SDT could be incorporated into faculty development and how performance can be optimised.

As an example, we can consider practitioners’ quality of motivation to gain patients’ consent. Gaining informed consent is a fundamental part of working as a health professional [38]. We know that patients are not optimally providing informed consent for procedures [39], nor for participating in medical research (e.g. pharmaceutical trials) [40]. There are acknowledged issues related to patients’ level of health literacy and clinicians’ overconfidence that patients have understood what they have explained, and there is an opportunity to examine the role of education and performance enhancement in addressing these issues [39]. SDT could be used to examine health professionals’ quality of motivation for gaining informed consent. Relevant, preliminary research questions include the following: “What is the quality of motivation that health professions students and health professionals demonstrate in relation to the technical and behavioural skills of gaining informed consent from patients” and “What influences health professionals’ quality of motivation for gaining consent?”.

When considering how to apply this knowledge into the design of a simulation, we can ask questions about the impact of different approaches for learning about the consent process. “Is externally regulated motivation to gain informed consent related to learning about this process from a predominantly legal perspective?” “Does learning/reflecting on these skills from a bioethics perspective lead to identified or integrated motivation when gaining consent in a simulated scenario?” “What are the intended and un-intended consequences for participants who have come to simulations from these different teaching perspectives?” Given previous work with SDT, we might hypothesise that learners will be impacted by these external factors, and their subsequent behaviours may be moderated by the lens of teaching or debriefing that is adopted. This same principle would apply to an array of technical and behavioural skills — hand hygiene, breaking bad news, engaging in low dose and high-frequency simulation for the maintenance of various skills.

Pathways exist for investigating the relevance of SDT to health simulation and for testing SDT theory in simulated contexts. These can be shaped to further extend the work of others who have investigated SDT and to provide evidence to underpin the various techniques and modalities of health simulation.

Ultimately, we should be aiming to generate and then to use the best available evidence to support simulation practice, support the refinement of learning outcomes and support faculty development efforts. SDT is a theory that has been built and tested slowly, strategically and with care not to oversimplify concepts or to foster reductionism. What we can work towards is not just isolated studies that may lead to another set of education myths [41, 42]. We have the opportunity to continue in the SDT tradition of systematically testing ideas and theory to determine if and what principles will facilitate a maturing of health simulation for teaching, training, systems testing, performance evaluation and professional development.

Conclusion

SDT is a theory that has been explored in many fields, and whilst elements have been explored in simulation, this exploration is in its infancy. Proposed in this paper is a rationale for conducting research that examines the relevance of the theory to health simulation and explore how health simulation may benefit from SDT research from other fields.

Why might we do this? We come back to the introduction of this paper where we consider the statements and philosophy that we want to be true in the field of health simulation. There is a pathway for testing our underlying assumptions and to enhancing our practice through detailed, structured and theoretically sound methods. In testing potential associations between SDT and simulation, we may be better informed about when statements we make are more likely to be true (“this is a psychologically safe environment”) and when they really may not be. In examining health simulation through the lens of SDT, we have opportunities to capture new insights into why simulation can be effective in enhancing performance and to further generate an evidence base for best practice in this field.