Introduction

Since the arrival of television sets in homes, each generation has faced the dilemma of how much time children should be allowed to watch them. As technology evolved, this problem has expanded to other electronic screen devices. Today, screen-time includes the time a child spends watching television, playing video games, browsing the internet, or using any mobile or tablet device. Such media can be used to deliver educational content or to promote positive behavior like good manners, sharing, forgiveness, and others which are beneficial to a child. The inappropriate and excessive use of such devices, however, has been related to harmful consequences (Canadian Paediatrics Society, 2017; Stiglic & Viner, 2019).

The American Academy of Pediatrics Council on Communication and Media (2013) has recommended 2 h/day as the maximum screen-time allowed for children and teenagers, and for children under two, no screen-time exposure was recommended. Children using screen devices for more than this recommended time have been found to be at a higher risk for developing several serious conditions. The association of excessive screen-time with obesity has been reported in Jordan (Al-Dalaeen and Al-Domi, 2017), other countries in the region like Qatar (Bener, 2011) and Saudi Arabia (Al-Ghamdi, 2013), and globally in China (Zhu et al., 2018), Sweden (Garmy et al., 2018), and the US (Kenney & Gortmaker, 2017), among others. Too much screen-time was related to abnormal sleeping patterns (Nathanson & Beyens, 2018; Twenge et al., 2017), increased risk of developing asthma and other allergies (Mitchell et al., 2013), and poor performance on developmental screening tests (Madigan et al., 2019).

Not only have studies indicated physiological associations, but also a correlation between a high level of screen-time and psychosocial problems in children (Khan et al., 2018; Parent et al., 2016), the subsequent development of attention problems (Swing et al., 2010), and poor school performance (Faught et al., 2017; Sharif & Sargent, 2006). With regard to acquiring knowledge, children below the age of three who were learning from screen devices were found to have difficulty transferring what they have learned from the 2D context (the screen) to 3D practice in real life (Barr, 2010).

The effect of screen-time on children’s health, well-being, and personal development, therefore, cannot be overlooked. In Jordan, 98% of households own a mobile phone (Department of Statistics/Jordan and ICF, 2019). In the US, 95% of families possess a smartphone (Wolf et al., 2018). Various methods have been tried to limit children’s screen-time. Schmidt et al. (2012) reported the success of feedback reward systems in reducing screen-time. In this method, children were awarded screen-time according to their physical activity. This metanalysis also reported that the use of television-monitoring devices also effectively lowered screen-time by electronically limiting the time allowed for children to watch television. Other programs encouraged physical activity, like dancing (Robinson et al., 2010) and playing video games that encouraged physical activity (Lanningham-Foster et al., 2006). Sherwood et al. (2019) recruited families from the pediatrics primary care clinics. These were given a brief primary care counseling about the prevention of obesity in the clinic followed by several, at home, phoned-based training. This had no significant effect on screen-time. A mixed approach was used by others (Habib-Mourad et al., 2014; Paw et al., 2008) to promote healthy behaviors. This included classes presented to children in their schools, meetings with families, giving handouts to families, and providing healthy food for school cafeteria. Despite this multi-component approach, no effect on screen-time was recorded. Harrison et al. (2006) conducted educational classes in schools with some success. Buscemi et al. (2019) used educational programs that aimed at preventing obesity including the relevance of reducing screen-time with no effect. In Jordan, no intervention programs were previously employed to reduce children’s screen-time. Two metanalyses (Schmidt et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2016), found that intervention programs were effective in reducing screen-time. This depended on the duration of the program and the method employed. Schmidt et al. (2012) found that educational programs delivered at the clinic were highly effective. Therefore, and because it was the most logistically feasible, this method was chosen for our research.

Reading is one activity that can be used to reduce screen-time (Khan et al., 2017), and a reading plan suitable for a particular culture can be easily designed. Reading to children or encouraging them to read on their own is one of the most fruitful behaviors, and has been shown to have a positive effect on their cognitive abilities (Murray & Egan, 2014). Moreover, promoting reading to preschoolers was shown to enhance their vocabulary (Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000), and school children who were better at reading during their initial school years, had a higher chance of graduating high-school and even enrolling in college (Lesnick et al., 2010). Furthermore, finding the right way to motivate children to read by themselves enhances their ability to comprehend what they read (Wigfield et al., 2016).

On the other hand, watching television has been shown to reduce the time children read books (Beentjes & van der Voort, 1989; Koolstra & van der Voort, 1996). This could be due to the time taken up by television viewing or it may be due to the content of the programs. Interactive media (like those found on today’s mobile devices) may adversely affect the concentration of children while reading (Wang et al., 2019). The use of e-books instead of paper books was found to be associated with less verbalization while reading by both parents and children (Munzer et al., 2019) and less warm interactions between parent and child (Yuill & Martin, 2016). So, encouraging parents to read to their children at a young age, especially printed books, and finding the proper motivation for older children to read by themselves can have great benefits to those children, including potentially providing a healthy substitute for screen-time.

Therefore, this research aimed to assess whether pediatric primary educational programs provided to parents were effective in reducing the screen-time of children in Jordan and enhancing their reading habits. In addition, the study aimed to assess if there was a correlation between screen-time and reading time.

Methods

During the months of October and November 2017, families seeking services from the pediatric out-patient clinic in a large public hospital in Amman, Jordan, were asked if they wanted to participate in pediatric primary prevention classes during their idle waiting time. These classes were prepared by a pediatrician member of this research team using evidence-based references. A group of qualified pediatricians and community medicine specialist reviewed these lectures and gave their feedback. The authors studied the feedback and modified the lectures accordingly until a final version was approved. A member of the team who was fluent in English and Arabic translated the material into Arabic (the native language of the participants). When the translated version was approved by the entire team, the lectures were made into Microsoft PowerPoint presentations to be used in the actual educational classes. The classes were made to take about 20 min. This was done because the participants were waiting to be called for their doctor’s appointment and making the classes longer might have deterred them from participating.

The authors who are fluent in Arabic all participated in the presentation of the classes to the participants. To help with this task, fourth and fifth year medical students were enrolled. These were divided into teams and given the materials to study. The authors gave several training sessions to these teams on how to present a lecture and how to be prepared to answer questions by the attendees. The training was held in lecture halls either in the university campus or in the hospital where the research was done. The student teams that were approved by the authors were allowed to give the educational classes.

There were three different evidence-based classes: (1) child nutrition, (2) child injury prevention, and (3) child developmental milestones. The families could choose to participate in any, all, or none of the classes offered. The classes were held in a lecture hall close to the out-patient clinic and on specified times according to a predetermined time-table. When the time of the class came, the families participating in that class were guided by medical students to the lecture hall. About 15–20 people were allowed in each class to make it easier for the participants to listen to the lecture and ask questions. In the child development class, the major developmental milestones were explained according to the various ages of children (less than 1 year, 1–3 years, 3–5 years, and 5–10 years). Also, the parents were given some simple points that may indicate abnormal development in their children. In addition, the main effects of excessive screen-time on children were briefly explained in this class and the recommended guidelines were given and alternative activities suggested. Concerning reading, the main benefits of reading to children starting from a very young age were emphasized. Pamphlets (in Arabic) were given to the families after the class. These contained a summary of the main points discussed in the class.

Six months after the completion of the classes (May/June 2018), participating families were surveyed, through telephone, specifically about the subjects of screen-time and reading habits to assess their adoption of the recommendations given to them during the classes. A short survey questionnaire was developed and discussed by three academics in the field to establish face validity. Back translation (Brislin, 1970) was used to develop an Arabic version of the questionnaire. For the purposes of this study, the families were divided into two groups: (1) the test group—included the families that participated in the developmental milestones class that included evidence-based materials on screen-time and reading habits for children, and (2) the control group—included the families that did not participate in the developmental milestones class, but that participated in one of the other prevention classes, and therefore were not exposed to the evidence-based materials on screen-time and reading.

Both groups were surveyed on the following questions:

Regarding screen-time:

  1. 1)

    How many hours per day does your child use screen devices (television, mobile, tablet, or computer) during weekdays?

  2. 2)

    How many hours per day does your child use screen devices (television, mobile, tablet, or computer) during weekends?

  3. 3)

    How many hours per day do you think it is appropriate for your child to use screen devices?

Regarding reading habits:

  1. 1)

    In your opinion, what is the appropriate age to start reading to your child?

  2. 2)

    How much time do you spend reading to your child or your child spends reading in a day? (asking specifically about non-school-related reading).

The average daily screen-time was determined using a weighted average of weekdays (5 days) and weekend (2 days) screen-times. For each family surveyed, the following demographics were recorded: the age of the parent, the number of children in the family, and the age of the children.

The data obtained were tabulated and analyzed using Microsoft Excel and IBM SPSS version 25 software and the two groups were statistically compared using t-tests. To find if there was a correlation between screen-time and reading time in each of the groups, a Pearson product–moment correlation was used.

Results

Groups

The test group included a total of 56 families. The control group included a total of 58 families. From each group, only 30 families responded to our calls during the follow-up survey. The rest were unable to be reached, either because they did not provide their contact number when they enrolled in the prevention classes, or they did not answer their phones when called.

Confounding factors

Compliance with guidelines presented in the primary prevention program could have been related to several potential factors, besides those being measured. Three demographic factors were considered in this research: age of parent, number of children in the family, and the age of the children. To assess the differences, if any, between the two groups an independent samples t-test was conducted. The results showed that the two groups did not display any significant differences in the mean values of any of these factors, and the two groups could, thus, be considered homogeneous with regard to these three factors (see Table 1).

Table 1 Differences between the two groups with regard to the confounding factors

Screen-time

The three screen-time questions were individually examined using an independent samples t-test. The average daily screen-time was calculated as a weighted average and then analyzed. The time children in the families from the test group spent on screen devices during weekends (M = 3.70 h) was significantly different from that of children from the control group (M = 5.15 h). No difference was, however, seen when looking at the weekdays’ screen-time, or when considering the weighted average for the whole week (see Table 2).

Table 2 Differences between the two groups with regard to screen-time

Looking at the mean screen-time during weekdays, weekends, and the average screen-time, they were higher than the recommended 2 h/day, in both groups. Based on the average screen-time, the percentage of children who were, actually, limited to a maximum screen-time of 2 h/day was only 37% for those whose parents had participated in the developmental milestones class (test group), but this was still more than the 17% for the control group.

When asked about the ideal screen-time for children, the test group reported an average of 1.60 h (SD = 0.87, n = 30), while the control group reported an average of 1.88 h (SD = 1.68, n = 28). When these means were compared to the recommended 2 h/day (using a one-sample t-test), an interesting finding emerged: the mean of the test group was significantly different from the recommended threshold, t(29) = − 2.53, p = 0.017, 95% CI [− 0.72, − 0.08]; whereas, the mean of the control group was not significantly different from the recommended 2 h/day, t(27) =  − 0.39, p = 0.697, 95% CI [− 0.78, 0.53]. This suggests that the parents in the test group were more aware of the need to limit the daily screen-time to below the recommended threshold.

Reading habits

For this aspect of the study, the families from the test and control groups were surveyed on the two reading habits questions and the responses were then analyzed using an independent samples t-test. The results showed that the families in the test group believed that they should start reading to their children at about 3 years of age compared to just over 4 years for the controls (M = 2.98, SD = 1.79; and M = 4.20, SD = 2.24, respectively). Children’s reading time (the time parents read to their children and/or the time children spent reading) was about 43 min/day for the children in the test group (M = 0.72 h, SD = 0.82), compared to about 16 min/day for the control parents (M = 0.27 h, SD = 0.44). Analysis showed that the differences were statistically significant for both of these aspects of reading habits (see Table 3).

Table 3 Differences between the two groups with regard to reading habits

Screen-time–reading time correlation

To find if there was a relationship between screen-time and reading time, a Pearson product–moment correlation was run. For the test group (n = 30), no significant correlation was found between these two factors, r(28) = 0.11, p = 0.562. This test also yielded no correlation between screen-time and reading time for the control group (n = 30), r(28) =  − 0.06, p = 0.766.

Discussion

For both the test and control groups, the majority of children used screen devices for more than the recommended 2 h a day (63% of families in the test group with a mean screen-time of 3.14, and 83% of families in the control group with a mean screen-time of 3.98). In a previous study in Jordan among teenagers aged 14–18 years, the percentage of those who used screen devices above the recommendation was 96% with a mean screen-time of about 7 h/day (Tayyem et al., 2014). A high screen-time of 6 h/day was also reported in school children aged 14–19 in Saudi Arabia (Al-Hazzaa et al., 2012), and 5 h/day for school children in Lebanon (Chacar & Salameh, 2011). In the US, in 2008, the percentage of children aged 4–11 who crossed the screen-time threshold was 65% (Anderson et al., 2008), and, in 2013, it was 46% for children aged 6–11 (Fakhouri et al., 2013). In Australia, it was 41% for preschool children (Hinkley et al., 2012), and, in China, 26% of adolescents aged 11 to 16 years reported screen-time of more than 2 h/day (Cao et al., 2011).

Although the percentages differed from study to study and from country to country, they all failed to meet the expectations of the recommended guidelines. Despite being lower than the previous studies in Jordan, Saudi Arabia, and Lebanon, the findings obtained in this study for children’s screen-time in Jordan were very high. Possible reasons include sedentary lifestyles, excessive use of computers for homework, and the absence of other perceived alternatives for spending free time such as reading (as this study results indicated children spent less than 1 h a day reading). Another important factor is that children may consider mobile devices as a means to be in semi-constant contact with their friends through messages and social networks, and, at the same time, be independent from their parents (Ling & Haddon, 2008).

Some demographic factors were compared across the groups due to their potential influence on the parent’s motivation to follow the recommended guidelines. The age of the parents may give us an indication of their level of education. The younger the parents, the more likely they are to be motivated to follow healthy guidelines for their children. The number and age of children in the family may indicate how busy the parents are, as more children could potentially limit the time parents could devote to each individual child, and children of different ages need different amounts of attention. No significant difference was, however, found between the two groups regarding these factors, and this would support the generalizability of the findings.

As to whether the primary prevention educational program impacted the families and their subsequent behavior: those that participated in the class which addressed screen-time and reading habits showed better understanding and compliance with the guidelines than those that had not. However, as to behavior modification, the only significant difference was found in the screen-time during the weekends. No such difference was found during weekdays. Part of this difference in screen-time between weekdays and weekends might be due to normal differences in family schedule between the weekdays and weekends. During weekdays, which are school/work days, parents might not impose restrictions on their children’s use of screen devices to do homework or for entertainment/play during their short evening free times, or the parents might not have the time or the motivation to monitor their children’s behavior after work; moreover, screen devices may even be used as a way to put the children out of the parents’ way. During weekends, on the other hand, children have more free time and the parents have more time and energy to engage their children and organize healthier alternative activities, like going out with the family or letting the children play outside with their friends. Furthermore, when considering the average number of hours per day a child spent on screen devices aggregated for the whole week (weighted average), the difference was masked and no statistically significant difference was found.

When asked about the ideal screen-time for children, both groups reported a mean of less than 2 h. When these means were compared to the recommended 2 h/day, only the mean of the test group was significantly different from the recommended threshold. This may indicate that the class the parents in the test group participated in had affected their knowledge regarding the importance of reducing their children’s screen-time below the threshold. Yet, this was not enough to fully reduce screen-time to the recommended guidelines, thus showing that knowledge, alone, did not translate into an effective behavior modification.

Various preventive methods regarding excessive screen-time were used over the years. Some were obesity prevention programs and some targeted screen-time specifically. Educating the parents was found to be the most effective method in reducing children’s screen-time in several studies (Bleakley et al., 2013; Harrison et al., 2006; Schmidt et al., 2012). Still, other methods should be implemented to reinforce knowledge and enhance outcome including: long-term programs with follow-up classes, the use of messaging services (SMS for example) to send educational messages (Shapiro et al., 2008), and the employment of active screen-time programs, like the use of video games that require physical activity (Lanningham-Foster et al., 2006).

The other area addressed by this study was the reading habits of children. Although the children of the families in the test group read (or were being read to) more than the children in the control group, the mean reading time was still less than 1 h/day for both groups. Why was reading time so low? How reading has been adopted in Middle Eastern culture should be addressed. A recent report documented that the limited availability of books in houses in Jordan may be one cause for the limited reading time (Department of Statistics/Jordan and ICF, 2019). The parents’ level of education and their interest in reading may also influence their children’s love for reading (van Bergen et al., 2017), as modeling plays a key role in child development. Another factor might be that schools focus on reading for homework and not on extracurricular or leisure reading, as one Jordanian study showed that reduced reading time was linked to homework overload and the lack of motivation by teachers and parents (Batainah & Al-Barakat, 2005).

In addition, the overuse of digital media can limit the time spent on reading. The data from this study showed that children from the test group spent 5 times more on their screen devices than on reading, compared to 16 times more for the control group. This is congruent with Banihani and Abu-Ashour (2015), who studied factors that affected reading habits of children in Jordan. In that study, they warned that digital media has captured much of the children’s time, and that outside reading is not at the top of a child’s preferred activities.

Parents, in both of our groups, were also under the impression that they should read to their children when they are older (about 3 years for our test group and about 4 years for our control group). Therefore, more comprehensive educational programs need to be developed to stress the importance of reading for the cognitive development of children, and to encourage parents to associate their children with books from infancy.

As shown in the findings, the calculation of a Pearson product–moment correlation from our data showed no significant correlation between screen-time and reading time for either group. This is similar to findings by Vandewater et al. (2006), but in contrast to several others who did find a relationship between screen-time and reading times (Khan et al., 2017; Koolstra & van der Voort, 1996). This inability to find a correlation could have been due to the small sample size of the groups, or due to some factors that were not considered in this study.

Limitations

The information about the children was obtained from their parents and, thus, might not have reflected the actual behavior of those children. Direct child interview or behavior monitoring might have been more appropriate, but this could not be implemented in this study. Despite our best efforts, we were only able to encourage few families to participate in our programs, and thus obtained only 30 families for each group. This might have led to sampling error. In addition, although we measured three factors that we thought might affect our study variables, there could have been some other potentially effective confounding factors that were not considered.

Conclusion and recommendations

The results from the current findings in Jordan are fairly congruent with studies elsewhere that have shown the increasing global trend of screen device overuse among children, but they also indicated some possible remedy. The use of pediatric primary prevention programs to educate families can contribute to improving parental knowledge of appropriate thresholds for children’s screen-time and desirable reading habits. Short, simple programs raise awareness about the guidelines and produce some behavioral changes, but they are not sufficient to make the parents fully adopt the recommended guidelines.

Comprehensive long-term programs may be required to achieve the desired goals and/or other methods may need to be employed to provide and reinforce the necessary educational material or to motivate parents/children to adopt a healthier behavior. Further research into reading is recommended to explore the differences between weekday/weekend reading, parent–child/child only reading, and paper/digital book reading.

The study of all aspects of a child’s life-style behaviors (screen-time, reading habits, physical activity, eating habits, etc.) as a whole will give us an even better understanding of the effects these factors have on the child’s health and future development, for to fully understand the mountain, it is not enough to examine only a single cave.