In this study, we demonstrated that all patients underwent revision rhinoplasty experienced a significant degree of satisfaction. The mean difference between preoperative and postoperative ROE satisfaction scores was 29.59, which is higher than the 16 outcome achieved by Hellings and Nolst Trenite (2007) (42.8 before surgery and 58.8 after surgery). We think that the biggest differences between the preoperative and postoperative scores in our study are associated with the shorter mean follow-up time (13 vs. 30 months). Longer follow-up periods may be associated with late complications such as graft loss, nasal collapse, synechiae, and so on (Thomas and Tardy 1986). However, we found no significant relationship between the follow-up period and the satisfaction scores in line with the findings of Arima et al. (2011, 2012).
We found that the improvement in ROE satisfaction scores was significantly higher in primary rhinoplasty patients in comparison with revision patients. Taking into consideration the inherent technical difficulties of revision rhinoplasty, the satisfaction scores of the revision group can be considered high, and even comparable with those of primary rhinoplasty patients.
We documented that younger patients tend to have less satisfaction increment after the revision. This is because these patients may have higher expectations as regards to their postoperative results (Arima et al. 2012). For this reason, we think that this group of patients need to be evaluated more comprehensively in preoperative setting, with more detailed information about the limitations of the surgery.
In this study, we found that male patients were significantly more likely to be dissatisfied than were female patients in agreement with the findings of Gorney and Martello (1999) and Khansa et al. (2016). It has been shown that male rhinoplasty patients have a poorer understanding of their deformity than do women (Wright 1987). In addition, men generally tend to have difficulties in verbalizing the morphologic or functional reasons for their dissatisfaction with the results (Khansa et al. 2016). These findings make it even more significant that the surgeon determines the male patient’s expectations and establish whether they are realistic during the preoperative consultation.
In this study, we found no relationship between the improvements in ROE satisfaction scores and the presence of mental illness. We believe that this finding is associated with low rate of psychiatric comorbidity in our patients. In the revision group, only 3 patients (5 %) were found to be positive for a mental illness. This rate is much lower than the previously reported rates (20–48 %) (Ishigooka et al. 1998; Sarwer et al. 2004).
Whatever the indication for surgery (aesthetic or combination of aesthetic and functional reasons), revision rhinoplasty increased patient satisfaction with highest scores obtained in patients with only aesthetic demands. Actually, we think that such a distinction is absolutely artificial because the aesthetics and function of the nose are inseparably related (Hahn and Becker 2014). For example, aesthetic complaints such as narrowing of the middle vault will generally present concurrently with obstructive symptoms. In this study, we excluded patients seeking revision for only functional reasons because the ROE questionnaire evaluates mainly the aesthetic aspects of rhinoplasty.
In revision rhinoplasty, the use of grafts is indispensable when large amounts of tissue are required (Bussi et al. 2013). Auricular or costal cartilage grafts had been used in nearly one-third of our revision patients. The improvements in ROE satisfaction scores were similar in both grafted and non-grafted patients. Considering that grafting is generally needed in severe nasal deformities, the relatively high rates of satisfaction in grafted patients indicate the functional and cosmetic benefits of grafting. For this reason, surgeons should not hesitate to use grafts if needed. However, we think that longer follow-up time periods are needed to determine the actual satisfaction state in grafted patients.
In the current study, approximately two-thirds of the revision patients reported satisfaction with the information they received in the preoperative visit. This situation was associated with higher rates of satisfaction with the postoperative results. We believe that providing patients with a satisfactory degree of information about the goals, limitations and possible complications of the surgery is fundamental for the exploration of expectations, motivations and perceptions. This is an important function in identifying patients who would benefit from the revision. On the other hand, we did not find any significant relation between the degree of satisfaction with the provided care and the increments in ROE satisfaction scores, which may be attributed to the low percentage of dissatisfied patients with the provided care.
We found no correlation between the severity of preoperative nasal deformity, measured by the subjective evaluation of nasal shape, and the improvements in satisfaction scores. For this reason, we think that even minor nasal deformities should be dealt with seriousness.
This study has some limitations which have to be pointed out. First, such surgeon-initiated questionnaires can be biased in favor of the surgery, because patients may be reluctant to express their dissatisfaction to their surgeons (Lee and Most 2016). We attempted to reduce this bias by asking patients to complete the ROE questionnaire anonymously. We think that the best way to increase the objectivity of such a patient reported outcome study is to conduct the survey by an independent researcher other than the operating surgeon. Second, single-center studies frequently lack the external validity required to generalize the results to a broader population. This is because operative techniques and follow-up protocols after revision rhinoplasty vary widely among surgeons. However, our findings represent a starting point for the evaluation of patient satisfaction after revision rhinoplasty. They allow larger controlled multi-center studies to be planned appropriately in order to include a wider range of population groups and to compare results among centers, all of which increase the generalizability of the results. Third, the design of this study did not allow us to assess the psychological factors that may have an influence on patient satisfaction. However, previous studies showed that the best candidates for rhinoplasty are psychologically stable patients who have requests focused on a specific physical feature (Vuyk and Zijlker 1995). Fourth, the six patients who were called but did not agree to participate in the study could have affected the average gain in satisfaction scores. These patients may not be interested in further evaluation because of their satisfaction or dissatisfaction with the results.