Background

Psychological factors, such as anxiety and depressive mood, have been reported to have influence on the eating habits of patients with life-style diseases, such as diabetes, which raises the risks of cardiovascular diseases [1]. Many studies have used questionnaires to investigate the association between psychological factors and eating habits, as in this previous study. However, the use of questionnaires is problematic because of their potential for recall bias [2]. To overcome this disadvantage, ecological momentary aqssessment (EMA), which avoids recall bias, has been used in the area of behavioral medicine to record subjective symptoms and behavior in subjects’ daily lives.

Some previous studies have been done to investigate the association between psychosocial factors and food intake using EMA [3,4,5,6]; however, these studies included methodological problems. First, most [3,4,5] used paper-and-pencil diaries to record subjective symptoms and food intake, while Tomiyama et al. [6] used an electronic diary (i.e. computerized EMA). Compared with EMA using paper-and-pencil diaries, computerized EMA has the advantage of avoiding ‘faked compliance’, i.e. disguised compliance by recording data at times other than those designated, by recording the time of data input [7]. In addition, the study by Tomiyama, which used computerized EMA, only assessed two days.

The aim of the present study was to do a long-term, quantitative investigation of the influence of preceding psychological factors on calorie intake, using computerized EMA for six months.

Methods

Participants and procedures

The participants were nine outpatients with type 2 diabetes diagnosed according to the current criteria of the Japan Diabetes Society [8] (three men; median age = 49 years, range = 34 to 72) who also took part in another study [9]. Inclusion criteria were age ≥ 20 and an outpatient with type 2 diabetes at the University of Tokyo Hospital. Participants were excluded if they had HbA1c ≥ 8.4% (corresponding to 8.0% before converting to the NGSP value, which was the criterion for poorly controlled type 2 diabetes), had any active mental disorder, followed a restrictive diet with protein ≤0.5 g/kg/day or salt ≤5 g/day due to another disease, had cognitive impairment, or had severe complications that interfered with self-care activities. After providing written informed consent, participants were instructed to record daily dietary intake using a PDA-based food diary, SELFOOD [10], and to record mood states, such as anxiety and depression, on a personal digital assistant (PDA) (WS020SH, Sharp Corp, Osaka, Japan, 135 g, 50x116x17.9 mm, Windows Mobile 6.1) that was equipped with a 3.0-in color liquid crystal display and a touch panel manipulated by a finger. They were asked to record as soon as possible after eating for 6 consecutive months. Anthropometric measurements, including height, body weight, and blood pressure, were performed at the beginning of the study. Clinical data (e.g. fasting plasma glucose level, hemoglobin A1c value, and duration of diabetes) were collected at their most recent visit with a health care provider. They visited their physician regularly, and continued to take the medications prescribed for their conditions during the study period.

Dietary intake

We developed an electronic food diary, “SELFOOD”, for use in a personal digital assistant (PDA). Its accuracy has been confirmed by comparison with a 24-h recall interview by registered dietitians [10]. SELFOOD includes a reference database with 423 color photographs of food and drink items and allows patients to add their own meals and recipes to the database if their nutrient information is given. SELFOOD calculates energy intake after every input. Calorie intake was summed on an input-by-input basis and converted to a daily bar graph with a horizontal line indicating the target value of daily calorie intake.

Momentary psychological stress and mood states

A PDA was used as electronic diary to record psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood [11]. The subjects were instructed on the use of the device and given manuals before the beginning of the study period. In addition, they practiced manipulating the device with the author until they became accustomed to its use.

The subjects used the PDAs for six consecutive months. Signal-contingent recordings were defined as recordings that were prompted with a beep as a signal [2], which was programmed to occur randomly within an interval of 30 mins, around 10:00 and 15:00. If the subjects did not enter a recording when the computer beeped, they were allowed to postpone input for 30 min. Recordings not made within 30 min were cancelled. The subjects were also asked to record their psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood when they woke up and when they went to bed by choosing “waking up” or “going to bed” from the menu. After selecting a “going to bed” recording, to avoid sleep disturbance the computers suspended the signal-contingent recordings until a selected “wake up” recording was played. Signal-contingent recordings and recordings when waking up and going to bed were treated as scheduled recordings. Momentary psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were rated with a visual analog scale (VAS) that ranged from 0 to 100, which was displayed on the screen.

Statistical analysis

Multilevel modeling was used for statistical analyses to investigate preceding psychological factors on dietary intake because the dataset in this study had a nested structure in which a number of recordings belonged to each participant. Pairs of preceding psychological factors and dietary intake were lagged in the range of − 5 to 0 h [12]. The level of significance was set at 0.05. SAS Proc Mixed (SAS 9.3, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) was used. Each dietary intake recording was paired with the preceding momentary recording of psychological stress and mood states. Dietary intake was treated as the dependent variable. Momentary psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were separately modeled as the independent variable, either as a fixed or random effect. Preceding momentary recordings of psychological factors were recorded within five hours before dietary intake, and there was no dietary intake between a preceding momentary recording of psychological factors and the dependent variable of dietary intake. Using these stratified time lags, models with different combinations of independent variables (with/without a preceding psychological factor as a fixed/random effect) were tested. Goodness of fit was compared to determine the best-fit model with a − 2 log likelihood function and χ2 test when one model was nested in the other; otherwise, Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) was used. The level-1 (within-individual level) intercept was modeled as a random effect. In all analyses, the variance-covariance matrix (G matrix) was modeled as unstructured.

Results

Subject characteristics

The characteristics of subjects are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Subject characteristics

Recording profiles

In total, the data from 1353 days of recordings was available for analysis. The compliance rate for regular dietary intake was 77.2%, while the compliance rate for signal-contingent recordings of psychological factors was 42.8%. With regard to pairing dietary intake and preceding psychological factors, 1517 of 3667 dietary intakes could be paired with preceding psychological factors.

Preceding psychological stress and dietary calorie intake

Preceding psychological stress was significantly, negatively associated with dietary intake for lunch and dinner, but positively associated for snacks (Tables 2, 3 and 4). The final model is as follows:

Table 2 Effect of preceding psychological stress on calorie intake, for lunch
Table 3 Effect of preceding psychological stress on calorie intake, for dinner
Table 4 Effect of preceding psychological stress on calorie intake, for snacks

Level 1 equation:

$$ {\mathrm{Y}}_{\mathrm{ij}}={\uppi}_{0\mathrm{i}}+{\uppi}_{1\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Company}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{2\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Place}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{3\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Stress}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\upvarepsilon}_{\mathrm{ij}} $$

Level 2 equation:

$$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}{\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\left({\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}++{\gamma}_{01}{\mathrm{TargetCal}}_{\mathrm{i}}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\mathrm{for}\ \mathrm{dinner}\right)\\ {}{\pi}_{1\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{10}\\ {}{\pi}_{2\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{20}\\ {}{\pi}_{3\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{30}\end{array}} $$

where Yij is each momentary calorie intake for the ith patient; Companyij is a corresponding accompanying person, recorded as alone or with someone; Placeij is the corresponding eating place; home, office/school, dining out, or other (e.g. outdoors); Stressij is the corresponding preceding psychological stress; TargetCali is the target calorie intake for the ith patient. γ00 is the average true value of mean calorie intake when all predictors are zero. π1i, π2i, and π3i are the individual i’s slopes representing the effect of an accompanying person(s), eating place(s), or preceding psychological stress on momentary calorie intake, respectively, and γ10, γ20, and γ30 are the average slopes. εij and ζ0i are residuals at each level. Including ζ0i in the equation means that the intercept was modeled as random, which suggests that intercept can vary across individuals. The second level 2 equation included no residual, which means that the effect of an accompanying person(s), eating place(s), or preceding psychological stress on momentary calorie intake was modeled as a fixed effect.

Preceding anxiety and dietary calorie intake

Preceding anxiety was significantly, negatively associated with dietary intake only for breakfast (Table 5). The final model is as follows:

Table 5 Effect of preceding anxiety on calorie intake, for breakfast

Level 1 equation:

$$ {\mathrm{Y}}_{\mathrm{ij}}={\uppi}_{0\mathrm{i}}+{\uppi}_{1\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Company}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{2\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Place}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{3\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Anxiety}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\upvarepsilon}_{\mathrm{ij}} $$

Level 2 equation:

$$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}{\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\\ {}{\pi}_{1\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{10}\\ {}{\pi}_{2\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{20}\\ {}{\pi}_{3\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{30}\end{array}} $$

where Yij is each momentary calorie intake for the ith patient; Companyij is a corresponding accompanying person, either alone or with someone; Placeij is the corresponding eating place, home, office/school, dining out, or other; Anxietyij is the corresponding preceding anxiety.

Preceding depressive mood and dietary calorie intake

Preceding depressive mood was significantly, negatively associated with dietary intake only for lunch (Table 6). The final model is as follows:

Table 6 Effect of preceding depressive mood on calorie intake, for lunch

Level 1 equation:

$$ {\mathrm{Y}}_{\mathrm{ij}}={\uppi}_{0\mathrm{i}}+{\uppi}_{1\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Company}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{2\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Place}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{3\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Depression}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\upvarepsilon}_{\mathrm{ij}} $$

Level 2 equation:

$$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}{\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\\ {}{\pi}_{1\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{10}\\ {}{\pi}_{2\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{20}\\ {}{\pi}_{3\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{30}\end{array}} $$

where Yij is each momentary calorie intake for the ith patient; Companyij is a corresponding accompanying person, either alone or with someone; Placeij is the corresponding eating place, home, office/school, dining out, or other; Depressionij is the corresponding preceding depressive mood.

Discussion

Using computerized EMA for 6 months, significant within-individual relationships between preceding psychological factors and calorie intake within 5 hours were shown in patients with type 2 diabetes. Although a causal relation could not be established, our results support the possibility that psychological factors can influence the calorie intake of patients with type 2 diabetes, which has not been investigated previously on a daily-life basis.

In the present study, the results showed differences in the association between preceding psychological factors and calorie intake among types of food. With regard to snacks, we showed that preceding psychological stress was positively associated with calorie intake. This result is consistent with the results of previous studies [3, 5, 6]. In the study by Lowe et al. [3], calorie intake from snacks increased when subjects with obesity were in a negative mood, although the study did not investigate the relation proportionally between negative mood and calorie intake. O’Connor et al. [5] reported a positive association between daily hassles and calorie intake from snacks, using only end-of-day assessments that did not include momentary psychological factors. Tomiyama et al. [6] used computerized EMA in a short two-day report that found preceding negative mood to be positively associated with calorie intake of snacks. Because the results of studies using such different methods were consistent, psychological stress would seem to increase calorie intake of snacks. Eating snacks may be a coping mechanism for reducing psychological stress [13].

With regard to regular meals, preceding psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were negatively associated with calorie intake. Negative mood may cause not only emotional eating, but appetite loss mediated by autonomic reaction. Because the patients were following diet therapy advice, the calorie intake in their regular meals might have been affected by a physical reaction rather than by an emotional reaction. The results of previous studies were not consistent regarding the association between psychological factors and calorie intake. Lowe et al. [3] found no association between negative mood and the calorie intake from regular meals, while Patel et al. [4] reported that the calorie intake from regular meals was greater for “negative mood” than for “neutral mood”. Because all of the subjects were women in the two previous studies, it would be difficult to compare them to the results of the present study. Therefore, future studies will necessary to confirm the association between preceding psychological factors and regular meals.

When controlling for psychological factors, calorie intake was generally greater when eating with than without another person(s) and when eating out than when eating at home. This result supports the results shown in a previous study that calorie intake increased in social situations [4].

There were some limitations in the present study. First, the sample size was small. Second, most of the patients in the present study were men. Therefore, future studies with more patients including more men and women will be necessary. Also, there was no control group of healthy people, which makes it impossible to expand the results in the present study to healthy people. In addition, some of the dietary intake could be paired with preceding psychological factors, which might be subject to selection bias.

Conclusions

Preceding psychological factors influence the calorie intake of patients with type 2 diabetes. Better understanding these factors will be helpful for developing psychological interventions to prevent overeating.