Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) provides the possibility to modulate cortical excitability of specific brain regions1,2, which can potentially modify a broad range of cognitive functions3,4,5,6, including attentional functioning7. Applying tDCS to improve and/or maintain performance gains special relevance in contexts where the targeted function is central to a broad range of tasks and degrades quickly over time. This is the case of vigilance, which requires sustaining the focus of attention over long time periods, and remaining alert to detect specific yet unpredictable stimuli8. Using tDCS to mitigate this inevitable decrement of vigilance over time has proven to serve as a fruitful intervention9,10,11,12. Specifically, anodal high-definition (HD) tDCS over the right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC)13,14 has shown to mitigate the decrement of executive vigilance (EV), understood as the ability to monitor and execute a specific response to infrequent but relevant stimuli15. Whereas it has shown no effect in mitigating the decrement in arousal vigilance (AV), understood as the ability to maintain a basic state of activation that allows responding to any stimuli of the environment in a fast and relatively automatic manner15.

A lateralization of sustained attention processes towards the right hemisphere has been established in neuroimaging studies16,17,18,19, as well as through lesion studies20,21. More specifically, lesion studies have identified the rPPC as a hub for spatial attention as well as vigilance22, whereas, on a functional level, the rPPC shows a heightened hemodynamic response to infrequently presented targets17, maintaining current task goals active as well as responding to (internal or external) novel stimuli23. This has led to considering the rPPC as a “convergence node” between the ventral attentional network and the default mode network (DMN), more associated with self-generated thoughts or mind-wandering24. Furthermore, imaging data from healthy participants suggests that the superior and inferior parietal cortices (constituting the rPPC) are densely interconnected forming a “structural core”25 that in turn is highly connected to other neural regions. This positions the rPPC as a highly relevant target for tDCS, given its functional relevance, as well as the potential benefit of tDCS effects spreading through relevant networks26,27. Considering the relevance of the rPPC in vigilance processes, the higher spatial precision achieved in the stimulated area by HD-tDCS, as compared to conventional tDCS28,29,30, is of special benefit for more precisely targeting this region.

To understand the underlying mechanisms of the vigilance decrement and its mitigation, one must consider that it may occur due to a complementary or alternative set of causes. Overload theories (resource-depletion hypothesis) assert that the vigilance decrement occurs due to the consumption of attentional resources with time-on-task due to the demanding nature of vigilance tasks31,32, with the associated experience of stress31,32,33,34. Other accounts (underload theories) posit that the underwhelming nature of vigilance tasks, more associated with boredom35,36, ultimately leads to a gradually more mindless execution of the task37,38. These theories can be tested empirically by manipulating cognitive demands (i.e., the number of simultaneous tasks to perform and therefore, task instructions to hold in working memory). Overload theories pose that increasing task demands would lead to a greater vigilance decrement, which has indeed been observed under normal conditions39,40,41 and found to be accentuated by sleep deprivation42. Underload theories, on the other hand, predict that lowering cognitive demands would lead to a less engaged and more mindless performance, steering thoughts away from the task’s goal43, producing the vigilance decrement44. Further support for underload theories stems from self-reported high mindlessness predicting worse performance in a vigilance task where targets appear with low frequency45, reports of task-induced physiological disengagement (i.e., parasympathetic activation and reduced cardiac reactivity)46, and activation of DMN structures with time-on-task47. Given this disparity of results, Thomson et al. propose the resource-control account, wherein resources are constant, but executive control declines with time-on-task causing the progressive shift of attentional resources from task-related towards task-unrelated thoughts (mind-wandering)48. This account considers that other factors than task demand can modulate the vigilance decrement: observing results such as a mitigated vigilance decrement with increased perceptual variability of the task’s target49, where higher difficulty demanding more resources is countered by higher engagement, possibly posing a smaller toll on executive control. Among other theories on the vigilance decrement (for a review see: Fortenbaugh et al.50), some accounts represent passive fatigue and active fatigue51 as two extremes on an inverse U-shaped function52 between performance and cognitive load53 or arousal54. These models incorporate both underload and overload as two extremes, between which we may attain a middle-ground of optimal performance53,54. As a case in point, Luna et al. created three load conditions (single task, dual task, and triple task) using the ANTI-Vea task15,55 and observed that the single and triple task groups showed a significant EV decrement, which was mitigated in the dual task group56. This further reinforced the view that the EV decrement, present with under and over-demand, is mitigated with intermediate cognitive demands.

The current understanding of how cognitive demands affect the vigilance decrement is still unclear given the disparity of findings39,40,41,44,46, and the current lack of models that explain diverging results. This is further obscured by the contradictory findings when using tDCS to modulate these effects11,57,58. A better understanding of cognitive load-dependent effects and their interaction with tDCS effects is needed for a better translation of these results towards applied fields. Critically, a more systematic modulation of task demands and stimulation parameters is required in order to define (i) which conditions lead to a greater vigilance decrement, and (ii) critically, under which conditions the vigilance decrement can be mitigated or reduced. The potential impact of these results can branch into (i) providing a small step towards research parameters to follow for understanding and mitigating the vigilance decrement, shedding some light on the currently often contradictory findings, (ii) adapting real-life contexts to optimize performance in human factor applications where the potential negative consequences of the vigilance decrement are greatest (e.g., air traffic control or security screening59,60), and (iii) provide the basis for constructing more efficient intervention or rehabilitation strategies for attention deficits such as those encountered in Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)61 or as a sequelae of stroke62, with better informed decisions on when to use compensatory strategies (e.g., reduce task demands to adapt to a lower threshold of what would be considered overdemanding) or restitutive approaches (e.g. training program where threshold of overdemand is increased with tDCS) during rehabilitation. In order to obtain a better roadmap for these outlined applications, further replications and, specifically, more systematic manipulations of cognitive load and tDCS is needed, which was the objective of the present study.

The present study

In the present study, we applied the task manipulations performed by Luna et al., measuring vigilance in a single and dual task56, in combination with HD-tDCS over the rPPC, following the same stimulation protocol as Hemmerich et al.13. Further comparisons were made with data from the original triple task studies (standard ANTI-Vea, of two previously collected samples13,14). This will allow (i) the replication of prior findings of cognitive load-dependent effects on the vigilance decrement56, and (ii) further understanding of whether/how these are affected by HD-tDCS. Given the specificity of HD-tDCS on the EV and not the AV effects13,14, and the differences in EV decrements depending on cognitive load56, we preregistered the following hypotheses (osf.io/9wfbx) regarding behavioural outcomes: (i) we expected a mitigated EV decrement (significantly reduced linear decrement of hits across task blocks in EV trials) in the anodal HD-tDCS group compared to the sham group performing the single load task, replicating the findings from Luna et al.56 in the sham group, and expecting the same beneficial effect of HD-tDCS in the anodal group that had been observed under higher cognitive load13, (ii) no EV decrement (no linear decrement) in the dual load task, expecting to replicate the findings from Luna et al.56, and therefore, no expected differences between stimulation conditions, and (iii) no modulation of AV performance (i.e., linear increment of SD of RT across blocks) in any load or stimulation group (replicating the specificity observed for the stimulation intervention for EV)13,14.

Methods

Participants

Participants (N = 120) were randomly assigned to perform a single or dual version of the ANTI-Vea task while receiving either sham or anodal HD-tDCS. The sample size of 30 participants per experimental condition matched those of prior studies with the standard ANTI-Vea with a priori estimated sample sizes13,14. See Table 1 for demographic data. All participants met the safety inclusion criteria for transcranial electrical stimulation (tES)63,64 and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were right-handed, and had no known neurological or psychiatric conditions. Participants signed an informed consent form and received monetary compensation for their participation (10€/hour). This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Granada (2442/CEIH/2021 and 1188/CEIH/2020), in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki (last update: Brazil, 2013).

Table 1 Sample sizes and demographic data for each experimental condition.

Apparatus and stimuli

Behavioural measures

Participants performed modified versions of the ANTI-Vea Task (as shown in Fig. 1B), where all trials of the standard task15 were presented, but task instructions and responses were coded differently. The ANTI-Vea task is an adapted version of the classical attentional networks task65, that includes independent measures of the executive and arousal vigilance components. For this purpose, the task is comprised of three types of trials (ANTI, EV, and AV) that are presented in pseudorandomized order. All ANTI-Vea versions used in this study were run for 7 blocks (560 trials in total). The ANTI trials (60% of total trials) allow measuring the functioning of the classical attentional networks (alerting, orienting, and executive control)66,67. These trials present a flanker task where the direction of the target (i.e., a central arrow) must be detected (pressing the c-key for left-pointing arrows, and m-key for right-pointing arrows) regardless of the direction of the flankers (i.e., surrounding arrows). The EV trials (20% of the total) prompt participants to detect an infrequent and large vertical displacement of the target of the flanker task, by giving an alternative response (pressing the space bar). This sub-task would be akin to signal-detection tasks such as the Mackworth Clock Test (MCT68). Lastly, AV trials (remaining 20% of trials) feature a red countdown (instead of the stimuli from ANTI or EV trials), which has to be stopped as fast as possible by pressing any key from the keyboard, akin to the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT69). For a more detailed description of the standard task and its parameters, please refer to: Luna et al.15, and Luna et al.55.

Figure 1
figure 1

ANTI-Vea Task procedure, electrode setup and resulting E-field simulation, and experimental procedure. (A) ANTI, EV, and AV targets of the ANTI-Vea task. The bottom table shows which target(s) participants are instructed to respond to (with a check) for the single, dual, and triple tasks. Note that perceptual load is maintained constant across all task conditions, as only instructions and response coding are modified. Note that both hands are placed over the keyboard at all times, using the left hand to press the “C” key and the right hand for the “M” key, whilst the “spacebar” key and the key chosen by the participant for AV trials can be pressed by any finger/hand (and must thus not be necessarily held constant). (B) Electrode setup for HD-tDCS: the anode is placed over P4 (red), and the surrounding cathodes over CP2, CP4, PO4, and PO8 (green), following the same protocol as Hemmerich et al. (C) Simulated voltage field obtained from the stimulation protocol from a top and right-hemisphere view. (D) Experimental procedure, where the bottom arrow shows the exact or approximate (preceded with a tilde) duration of each step, in minutes. Each fatigue assessment took less than a minute.

General task instructions across the different load conditions were given for participants to keep their gaze on the fixation point (“ + ”) in the centre of the screen and to respond as fast and as accurately as possible. Then, instructions diverged according to the manipulation of cognitive load, to reflect the correct response for each type of trial as depicted in Fig. 1A. While maintaining perceptual load constant, the manipulation of task instructions and response coding resulted in: (i) a single task, which required participants to respond only to EV trials, and (b) a dual task, in which participants had to respond to both EV and AV trials. These two groups were then further compared with data from (iii) a triple task, where participants had to respond to ANTI, EV, and AV trials (standard ANTI-Vea), collected from two previous studies13,14 (N = 120).

HD-tDCS setup

HD-tDCS was applied with a Starstim 8® device and hybrid NG Pistim Electrodes (Ag/AgCl, contact area: 3.14 cm2) controlled through NIC v20.6 software (Neuroelectrics®, Barcelona). Five of the electrodes, placed in a neoprene headcap, were set up in a 4 × 1 ring-like array, targeting the rPPC by placing the central anode over P4, and the four surrounding cathodes over CP2, CP6, PO4, and PO8 (see Fig. 1B and C). Using a single-blind procedure, anodal (1.5 mA) or sham (0 mA) HD-tDCS was applied according to random group allocation, from the 2nd to the 6th task block (see Fig. 1D). The sham protocol consisted of two ramps (30 s ramp-up and 30 s ramp-down) at protocol onset and offset. The anodal protocol consisted of an initial ramp-up (30 s) followed by active stimulation (~ 28 min), and a ramp-down (30 s) at offset. In this study, electroencephalographic (EEG) signal was recorded during the 1st task block serving as a baseline, and during the 7th block, serving as a post-stimulation measure. Further details regarding this step are beyond the scope of this report as EEG data will not be presented.

Fatigue assessment

Subjective mental and physical fatigue ratings were assessed throughout the experiment: baseline, pre-task, and post-task (see procedure or Fig. 1D). Responses were recorded through a visual analogue scale: a horizontal line ranging from minimum (left side of the screen) to maximum fatigue (right side). The assessment order for fatigue type was counterbalanced across participants but kept constant for each participant’s session, following the procedure of Luna et al.56.

Procedure

As in Hemmerich et al. the experimental session began with an MRI scan13 (~ 28 min), mainly focused on acquiring diffusion-weighted imaging data. This data is being collected as part of a larger research project and will not be covered in the present report. Participants then sat in a separate, dimly lit room to complete the experiment. First, participants completed the baseline fatigue assessment and the ANTI-Vea’s practice blocks (adapted for each load condition). After electrode set-up, participants completed the pre-task fatigue assessment. Then the experimental task started, during which stimulation was applied from the 2nd to the 6th experimental block. Right after the completion of the last (i.e., 7th) experimental block, the post-task fatigue assessment and the tES Survey70 were completed.

Statistical analyses

Following the preregistered plan of analysis, we analysed EV and AV data from baseline (1st block) to the final active or sham stimulation block (6th), following prior HD-tDCS studies13,14. Following the standard approach to ANTI-Vea scores15, we computed EV indices [Hits (percentage of correct responses), False Alarms (FA), Sensitivity (A’), and Response Bias (B”)] and AV indices [mean RT and standard deviation of RT (SD of RT)]. For EV data, we compared baseline differences in EV indices between stimulation groups using an ANOVA. Then, each index was included in an ANOVA as a dependent variable, with Blocks (1st–6th) as a within-participant factor and Stimulation Group (anodal or sham HD-tDCS) and Task Load (single or dual) as between-participant factors, followed up by partial ANOVAs for each Task Load level. Polynomial contrasts were used to analyse the linear component of each index across Stimulation Group for each Task Load level. Then, the single and dual task data, combined as a not-triple condition, were re-analysed jointly with triple-task data13,14, combined as a triple condition, repeating the above-described analyses (with Updated Task Load) on two balanced samples (ntriple = 120, nnot-triple = 119). Lastly, results for AV data are reported first considering only low-load conditions (i.e., only dual task) and then comparing low and high-load conditions (i.e., dual vs triple task, using data from the present study and data from Hemmerich et al.13 to achieve comparable sample sizes in each group).

Note that for all reported ANOVAs, degrees of freedom are reported with Greenhouse–Geisser correction when the sphericity assumption was violated (i.e., p > 0.05 in Mauchly’s test). Additionally, across results, equivalent Bayesian tests are reported to further test the validity of our inferences, as a supplement to non-significant frequentist results. Note Bayes Factors in favour of the null hypothesis (BF01) provided for polynomial contrasts on the linear decrement correspond to independent or one sample t-tests completed on the Slope across Blocks (1st–6th).

Methods and Results for Subjective Mental and Physical Fatigue are reported in Appendices EH of the Supplementary Material.

Results

Blinding efficacy

The total amount of self-reported discomfort/sensations associated with stimulation70 was significantly different between the Stimulation Groups, U = 2190, p = 0.037, with higher discomfort reported in the sham (M = 2.43, SD = 2.08) than in the anodal (M = 1.68, SD = 1.85) group. This difference seems to be mainly driven by the significantly higher intensity reported for pinching in the sham group (M = 0.38, SD = 0.80) than in the anodal group (M = 0.03 SD = 0.18), U = 2166, p = 0.001, without any differences for the remaining sensations (all p’s > 0.136, see Appendix A of the Supplementary Material for further statistical details). The higher discomfort reported in the sham group likely led to a higher estimation of belonging to the active stimulation group in the sham (62%) than in the anodal group (42%). However, the guessed active group allocation was not statistically different between Stimulation Groups, χ2(2, N = 120) = 4.85, p = 0.088. Taken together with the evidence for group differences in total discomfort (BF10 = 1.07) and pinching (BF10 = 0.93) being anecdotal71 at most, leads us to conclude that blinding was still effective in the present study.

EV decrement under lower cognitive demands: single vs. dual cognitive load conditions

Following standard filtering for ANTI-Vea data55, outliers (defined based on accuracy < 50% in EV and/or AV trials), excluded one participant (sham-single) from further analyses. There were no significant differences in EV Hits at baseline (Block 1) between the sham and anodal HD-tDCS groups for the single task condition, F(1, 57) = 2.07, p = 0.156, ƞp2 = 0.04 (BF01 = 1.60), or the dual task condition, F < 1 (BF01 = 3.73). Similarly, no differences between the Stimulation Group at baseline (Block 1) were observed for EV A’ in the single task condition, F(1, 57) = 1.92, p = 0.172, ƞp2 = 0.03 (BF01 = 1.70), or the dual task condition, F(1, 58) = 1.20, p = 0.278, ƞp2 = 0.02 (BF01 = 2.31),

Regarding EV Hits, The Blocks × Stimulation Group × Task Load mixed ANOVA performed on Hits only showed a significant main effect of Blocks, F(3.72, 428.18) = 24.27, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.17. However, no interactions were significant: Blocks × Stimulation Group, F < 1, Blocks × Task Load, F < 1, Blocks × Stimulation Group × Task Load, F < 1 (all BFs01 > 38.27), as shown in Fig. 2A (Note that the reported results span Blocks 1–6, as per our pre-registered plan for analyses. Nonetheless, for clarity, repeating the analyses over Blocks 1–7 yielded the same result. For low-load conditions (single and dual task), the effect of Block remains significant, F(4.19, 481.69) = 23.55, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.17, without significant interactions (all F’s < 1)). A polynomial contrast showed that all groups (joint analysis across experimental conditions) had a significant linear decrement across time, F(1, 115) = 51.98, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.31. Importantly, in regard to our hypotheses, polynomial contrast showed the expected significant linear decrement of Hits across Blocks in the sham conditions of the single task, F(1, 57) = 8.42, p = 0.005, ƞp2 = 0.13, which, against our hypotheses was also observed in the sham condition of the dual task, F(1, 58) = 12.72, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.18. These linear decrements were not significantly different between the two Task Load conditions, F < 1 (BF01 = 3.25).

Figure 2
figure 2

(A) Mean % of Hits in EV trials across Blocks for single and dual cognitive load conditions. A linear decrement across Blocks was observed across all conditions. (B) Sensitivity (A’) in EV trials across Blocks for the single and dual cognitive load conditions. An effect of Blocks on A’ is observed regardless of the stimulation condition, although the linear component was not significant, whilst the single task condition shows a lower mean A’ (averaged across Blocks) in the anodal compared to the sham condition. Note. The dashed vertical line represents the onset of the stimulation protocol. The dotted line represents the offset of the stimulation protocol. The shaded ribbons represent the standard error of the mean (SEM).

Regarding sensitivity (A’) for EV trials, although a main effect of Blocks, F(4.34, 499.11) = 2.48, p = 0.031, ƞp2 = 0.02, was observed, polynomial contrasts show no significant linear decrement across Blocks (across all conditions), F(1, 115) = 1.39, p = 0.240, ƞp2 = 0.01 (BF01 = 4.89). More importantly, the effect on Blocks did not interact with Stimulation Condition, F < 1 (BF01 = 127.80), Task Type, F(4.34, 499.11) = 1.58, p = 0.174, ƞp2 = 0.01 (BF01 = 12.90), or an interaction of both F < 1 (BF01 = 55.47), as depicted in Fig. 2B. As can be observed from Fig. 2B, while the linear decrement is not different across conditions, in the Single Task condition, a difference in overall Hits and A’ can be observed. For mean % Hits (across Blocks 1st–6th) the difference between stimulation conditions did not reach significance, t(57) = − 1.88, p = 0.065 (BF01 = 0.88), whereas a significantly lower mean A’ (across Blocks 1–6) is observed in the sham single task condition (M = 0.94, SD = 0.03), compared to the anodal single task condition (M = 0.92, SD = 0.03), t(57) = − 2.76, p = 0.008. Refer to Appendix B of the Supplementary Material for further results on the remaining EV indices (FA and B”).

EV decrement under effects of increased cognitive load: single and dual cognitive load conditions vs. triple load

Baseline (i.e., 1st Block) Hits for EV trials were significantly lower for the triple task condition (M = 82%, SD = 15), compared to the single (M = 94%, SD = 8) and dual (M = 96%, SD = 7) conditions, F(2, 234) = 37.60, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.24. However, and importantly, within the triple task condition, there were no significant differences between Stimulation Groups, F < 1 (BF01 = 3.85). Similarly, no baseline differences were observed for EV A’, F < 1 (BF01 = 5.01).

The ANOVA performed on Hits in EV trials with Blocks as a within participants variable and Stimulation Group and Updated Task Load (triple/not-triple) as between-participant factors, reflected a main effect of Block, F(4.31, 513.24) = 21.42, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.15, which interacted significantly with Stimulation Group, F(4.31, 513.24) = 3.69, p = 0.005, ƞp2 = 0.03. Importantly, the three-way Blocks × Stimulation Group × Updated Task Load interaction was significant, F(4.24, 999.51) = 2.97, p = 0.017, ƞp2 = 0.01 (For transparency, to complement the pre-registered analyses over Blocks 1–6, repeating the same analyses across Blocks 1–7, yields the same results: the main effect of Block, F(5.01, 1181.07) = 44.78, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.16, and the critical three-way Block × Stim × Updated Task Type interaction remain significant, F(5.01, 1181.07) = 2.91, p = 0.013, ƞp2 = 0.01). Polynomial contrasts completed on the grouped (triple vs. not-triple) data showed that the linear decrement between the anodal and sham conditions was not different for the not-triple condition, F < 1 (BF01 = 4.09), whereas it was for the triple task condition, F(1, 119) = 8.62, p = 0.004, ƞp2 = 0.07. Bayesian analyses further showed that there was moderate evidence (BF10 = 5.66) for this mitigated EV decrement in the triple task anodal group, as can be seen in Fig. 3 (right), compared to extreme evidence (BF01 = 145.25) against a significant interaction in the not-triple task condition, as shown in Fig. 3 (left). Lastly, there was a significant difference in the linear decrement observed between sham conditions between the not-triple and triple tasks, F(1, 117) = 7.99, p = 0.006, ƞp2 = 0.06, reflecting, the significantly greater EV decrement under high compared to lower load conditions. In contrast, the anodal not-triple and triple conditions’ linear decrement were not significantly different from each other, F(1, 119) = 1.02, p = 0.316, ƞp2 = 0.01 (BF01 = 3.17), which indicates that HD-tDCS in the triple task conditions seems to mitigate the vigilance decrement up to the performance level observed for the single or dual task conditions.

Figure 3
figure 3

(A) Mean % of Hits and in EV trials across blocks combined for the single and dual task conditions (low-load, left), and triple task condition (high-load, right). The above-reported linear decrement in low load conditions, without an effect of HD-tDCS stands in contrast with a significantly lower linear decrement in the anodal compared to the sham HD-tDCS condition of the triple task. (B) Sensitivity (A’) in EV trials across Blocks for the low-load condition (left) and the high-load condition (right). In the sham triple task condition, a much steeper decrement of A’ is observed, compared to the non-significant linear component in the triple anodal condition, which is comparable to both low-load conditions. Note. The dashed vertical line represents the onset of the stimulation protocol. The dotted line represents the offset of the stimulation protocol. The shaded ribbons represent the SEM.

Notably, Sensitivity (A’) also decreased significantly across Blocks, F(4.59, 1084) = 3.82, p = 0.003, ƞp2 = 0.02, and was modulated by Stimulation Condition, F(4.59, 1084) = 2.72, p = 0.022, ƞp2 = 0.01, but not by Updated Task Load, F(4.59, 1084) = 2.05, p = 0.08, ƞp2 = 0.01 (BF01 = 16.71). Importantly, the triple interaction was significant, F(4.59, 1084) = 2.82, p = 0.019, ƞp2 = 0.01. Polynomial contrasts reflected a significant linear decrement in A’ in the triple task sham group, F(1, 119) = 23.36, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.16, significantly different from the linear decrement in the triple task anodal group, F(1, 119) = 12.11, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.09, where, notably, no significant linear decrement was observed, F < 1 (BF01 = 7.01), as can be seen in Fig. 3 (bottom panel, right). See Appendix C of the Supplementary Material for further results on the remaining indices for EV trials (FA and B”).

AV decrement: dual vs. triple load conditions

For the dual task AV data there were no significant baseline differences between the two Stimulation Groups on SD of RT, F < 1 (BF01 = 3.64). As predicted, there was a significant AV decrement, shown as an increment in the SD of RTs to AV trials across Blocks, F(3.39, 196.87) = 4.86, p = 0.002, ƞp2 = 0.08, which was not modulated by HD-tDCS, F < 1 (BF01 = 16.26) (To complement the pre-registered analyses over Blocks 1–6, if the same analyses are repeated over Blocks 1–7, the same results are observed: comparing the AV (SD of RT) across the dual and triple tasks also showed a significant effect of Blocks, Block: F(4.11, 476.88) = 11.23, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.09, but no significant interactions (p’s ≥ 0.145)). Polynomial contrasts further showed that whilst there was no significant linear increment in the sham group, F(1, 58) = 3.23, p = 0.077, ƞp2 = 0.05 (BF01 = 0.54), it was significant for the anodal group, F(1, 58) = 7.90, p = 0.007, ƞp2 = 0.12. Importantly, the linear increment was not significantly different between Stimulation Groups, F < 1 (BF01 = 3.07), as shown in Fig. 4.

Figure 4
figure 4

AV decrement (increment of SD of RT with time-on-task) as a function of stimulation condition for the dual task (left) and the triplet ask condition (right). No differences between the linear increment of SD of RT across Blocks were observed between Stimulation Groups of either task condition. Note. The dashed vertical line represents the onset of the stimulation protocol. The dotted line represents the offset of the stimulation protocol. The shaded ribbons represent the SEM.

Finally, an ANOVA performed on SD of RT, contrasting the dual and triple conditions, showed a significant AV decrement (increment of SD of RT) across Blocks, F(4.02, 466.80) = 9.32, p < 0.001, ƞp2 = 0.07. However, this did not interact with Stimulation Condition, F(4.02, 466.80) = 1.35, p = 0.249, ƞp2 = 0.071 (BF01 = 21.70), or Task Load, F(4.02, 466.80) = 11.62, p = 0.167, ƞp2 = 0.01 (BF01 = 12.10), nor was there a significant triple interaction, F < 1 (BF01 = 15.09), as can be seen in Fig. 4. Refer to Appendix D of the Supplementary Material for further AV results (Mean RT in AV trials).

Discussion

This study aimed at investigating the influence of cognitive load and HD-tDCS, as well as their interaction, on the EV decrement. To this end, we manipulated task load (single or dual) and HD-tDCS application over the rPPC (sham vs. active). Contrary to our preregistered hypotheses, we observed no differences between the EV decrement in the single and dual task conditions and no modulation of this decrement by HD-tDCS. As expected, neither cognitive load nor HD-tDCS modulated the AV decrement. Importantly, when contrasted with prior results using a triple task, we are able to expand evidence on the specific effect of rPPC HD-tDCS on the executive component of vigilance13,14: the mitigatory effect of HD-tDCS is only evident under conditions of high cognitive demand.

Against our pre-registered hypothesis, we did not replicate the findings of Luna et al.56, as the single and dual load conditions both showed a significant EV decrement with time-on-task, without any differences across load conditions. Some studies report similar null effects comparing single and dual tasks32,72, or no vigilance decrement at all regardless of the load condition73,74. However, most of the literature is either skewed towards underload (observing larger decrements with lower task demands44 or higher engagement75) or overload theories (observing greater vigilance decrements with increased task demands by adding a secondary task39,40,41 or increasing instruction complexity72), without any clear consensus. One possible explanation for our diverging results is that single and dual tasks yielded conditions that were qualitatively not sufficiently different and therefore processed similarly. Under these low to medium load conditions, available resources may suffice to (somewhat successfully) complete the task and mind-winder in parallel (maintaining the same level of performance across slightly differing demand conditions). This could be explained by the resource-control account, as executive control decreases with time-on-task, gradually tipping the balance from task-related towards task-unrelated thoughts48. The single and dual tasks may operate at a relatively low “tipping point”. Importantly, the EV decrement has been recently linked with the loss of executive control with time-on-task in the standard ANTI-Vea (triple task)76. Future research systematically manipulating task demands in a within-participants design could explore: (i) whether executive control measures and the EV decrement are related when task demands are reduced, and (ii) how each load level influences the presence of task-unrelated thoughts.

Contrary to the expected mitigated EV decrement in the single group receiving active HD-tDCS and no effect of HD-tDCS on EV performance in the dual group, we observed no mitigatory –or detrimental– effect of stimulation in either the single or dual task condition. Similar results have been observed with the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) comparable to our single task condition: prefrontal tDCS did not affect target accuracy57, and anodal or cathodal tDCS over the right inferior parietal cortex (rIPL) did not affect error rates or RTs77. Similarly, another study reports null effects of anodal tDCS over the left PFC in a dual working memory task58. However, there are also some reports of detrimental effects of higher doses of both anodal and cathodal tDCS over the rIPL on accuracy in the SART78, and beneficial effects on accuracy with anodal HD-tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) regardless of the task demand condition of a standard and a modified SART79. Lastly, it has been suggested that prefrontal tDCS may modulate sustained attention by affecting its higher-order sub-processes, rather than simple target detection7, which could partially explain the absence of effects of tDCS in low demanding conditions.

In contrast to the null effect of HD-tDCS on the EV decrement in the low and medium load conditions, the mitigatory effect of rPPC HD-tDCS was only observed in the most demanding condition (triple task). The EV decrement in the sham triple-task condition was more pronounced than under single and dual load, which was mitigated in the HD-tDCS condition. Similar results have been observed with anodal tDCS over the right DLPFC, leading to improved accuracy under the highest load condition of a working memory task80, and anodal tDCS over the left DLPFC leading to delayed beneficial effects on multitasking but not on single task performance81. Other studies also suggest that tDCS over right prefrontal or parietal areas can lead to detrimental effects on task performance under objective11 and subject-specific high load conditions82. In contrast, some studies have reported beneficial effects of cathodal tDCS for maintaining or improving performance in high load conditions83,84. Studies on the intersection of cognitive load and tDCS are still rather scarce and yield no clear conclusions. While the inconsistencies across the existing literature are partially explained by the variability between stimulation procedures, cognitive processes studied, and tasks used across these different studies, a crucial factor to consider is the conceptualization of cognitive load and how its levels are established. Roe et al. argue that “[…] using a load level that overtaxes cognitive capacity, as well as making use of a wider range of load levels (i.e., more than two), is preferable if one’s goal is to investigate the interaction between tDCS and cognitive load11. Precisely, the high load condition of our study, although complex and demanding, is not overtaxing, as was the case for the high load condition of studies reporting detrimental effects of anodal tDCS11,82. The effects of tDCS on the vigilance decrement are likely to depend less on the externally imposed and conceptualized levels of cognitive load, but rather on the specific demand they impose on each individual, and the specific neural state they induce85. Therefore, as illustrated in Fig. 5, high but manageable cognitive demands could lead to beneficial effects of anodal tDCS, as observed in the present study, where increasing neural excitability may further excite task-relevant processes. However, we hypothesize that when further increasing demands to a level where task performance cannot be maintained, the effects of anodal tDCS would be detrimental, as increasing the excitability of overtaxed neural circuits is likely to disrupt task performance. This might also explain facilitatory effects of cathodal tDCS in tasks with high demand83,84, where inhibitory processes could reduce over-demand. Lastly, in the lower load conditions (single and dual task), a ceiling effect of the modulatory effects of HD-tDCS may be taking place.

Figure 5
figure 5

Observed and hypothesized interaction of cognitive demands and HD-tDCS over rPPC on the accuracy performance with time-on-task (TOT), with lower values depicting a greater EV decrement. (A) Beneficial effect of active HD-tDCS over the rPPC, mitigating the EV decrement, as observed in the present study. (B) Further increasing task demands to a level that is overtaxing, would potentially lead to even worse EV performance, which could be further deteriorated by the application of active tDCS –as conceptualized and observed by Roe et al.11.

Another relevant result of the present study is the finding that performance gains, namely, the improved accuracy in target detection for EV trials, were due to improved sensitivity (i.e., ability to discriminate signal from noise), and not due to shifts in the response bias (i.e., the adoption of a more liberal response criterion, which would merely increase hits at the cost of increasing false alarms). While some studies do report similar results86,87, signal detection theory measures are not discussed in most studies exploring the effect of tDCS on vigilance, and opposite findings have also been reported showing greater sensitivity declines in less demanding tasks88. Thus, whilst requiring further replication, for now, our results highlight that when HD-tDCS mitigates the EV decrement (in high demand conditions), it does so by improving performance in a precise manner.

Taken together, our results further point to the fact that underlying mechanisms driving EV performance are not being properly explored with the tools at hand. As suggested above, a better understanding of what is causing the vigilance decrement, as would do, for example, collecting thought probes throughout the task, would help further understand the present results. Although future challenges still lie in the fact that the presence of mind-wandering is not a fool-proof sign of underload, as the presence of mind-wandering does not always predict performance costs89, nor does the manipulation of task demands always lead to different mind-wandering rates79. Future research could bridge this gap by including, not only thought-probes in vigilance tasks but also including other more objective measures of engagement, such as eye movements90. Finally, given that the vigilance decrement can be shaped by a myriad of different factors91, future research should refine their approach in studying cognitive load dependent effects on vigilance, in which considering individual differences should be a key factor.

However, despite the above-mentioned limitations and open questions, the present findings can tentatively inform future decisions in research and clinical settings. The cognitive-load dependent effects of HD-tDCS on the EV decrement as observed in the present study underline the importance of considering cognitive load as an essential factor in: (i) predicting stimulation outcomes, and (ii) tailoring the interactions of demands and tDCS individually. Regarding the first point, whilst future research is needed to understand the generalizability of these results, our data suggests that in areas where a tDCS intervention is to be applied but cognitive demands cannot be modified or adapted, a prediction (based on behavioural data) could be made as of how successful a tDCS intervention would actually be. If the task is overdemanding, the intervention is likely to not adequately induce plastic changes towards the desired outcomes, whereas, if the task is under-demanding, a ceiling effect might hamper any real efficacy of the stimulation as well. While prior to such applications, further research would be needed, this consideration could be a first step in more precisely delineating the intervention and, potentially, offer a broad guideline that could avoid devoting resources to null findings. Regarding the second point, when the cognitive demands can be individually assessed and adjusted to an optimal level, the efficacy of interventions focused on the rehabilitation of attentional functions could be greatly improved. In a clinical setting, attention deficits such as those elicited by ADHD61 or as a sequelae of a stroke62, could lead to the subjective and individual experience of high cognitive demands or even result in an over-taxing of resources in context that are considered to be of low demand under normal circumstances. Given that the threshold of what is considered overdemanding is not even uniform among healthy participants58,82, it will likely be even more heterogenous in these clinical populations. Therefore, instead of externally imposing a fixed demand, individually tailoring demand levels of cognitive training tasks to individual capacity58,82 and gradually increasing task demands, for online use in a tDCS intervention may ensure that the neuroplastic effect of tDCS actually reinforces effective task-resolution and learning processes85 as a restitutive approach to regain attentional functioning.

Conclusions

According to our results, the EV decrement does not seem to be modulated by cognitive load under relatively undemanding conditions (towards improved performance in the dual load group, as was reported by Luna et al.56). Indeed, both single and dual load conditions showed a similar vigilance decrement across time. Under these conditions (single and dual cognitive load), additionally, HD-tDCS does not affect EV performance. However, under conditions with higher demand (i.e., triple task) there is a steeper vigilance decrement compared to lower load conditions, which was mitigated via anodal HD-tDCS over the rPPC. This study highlights the fact that task demands should be an important factor in considering the efficacy of a tDCS intervention on vigilance performance. This will allow a better understanding of the vigilance decrement in itself and facilitate a more effective translation of these results into clinical settings.