Introduction

The identification and treatment of respiratory illness is a key challenge and major welfare concern for dairy calves1, prompting interest in behavioral changes coinciding with disease. Group-housed calves present an opportunity to understand changes in social networks and how they correspond to health. Studies of sickness behavior and the impact on social interaction patterns and network position have proven useful for wildlife disease ecology (e.g., vampire bats, Desmodus rotundus2; Tasmanian devils, Sarcophilus harrisii3). However, links between social networks and health are lacking for managed populations. In particular, there is limited understanding of how social relationships of gregarious livestock species may be related to health status.

Animals often respond to infection with cytokine-induced sickness behavior, which is considered to be a motivational state and includes fatigue, loss of appetite and social withdrawal4. Changes in social behavior have been characterized as a component of sickness behavior across group-living species. Both a reduction and increase in interactions have been observed; for example, wild mice (Mus musculus domesticus) limit the size of their social contact network in response to an infection5. Whereas in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), increases in affiliative interactions with familiar peers and kin have been reported following an experimentally induced infection6. Humans have also been shown to both increase7 and limit8,9,10 their social interactions while sick, depending on the context and availability of social partners. In dairy calves, we found previously that individual calves initiated less social grooming following a respiratory disease challenge11, suggesting disease may be associated with changes in social relationships in group-housed dairy calves. In dairy calves, it is established that lying behavior is sensitive to naturally occurring illness, even increasing prior to clinical signs of illness12. However, no research to date has examined pairwise social interactions at the group level and consequently the social networks of dairy calves following infection.

Network approaches are valuable because they examine indirect connections beyond the level of the dyad and offer novel ways to uncover association between social behavior and animal health/welfare. Social network analysis (SNA) is a quantitative framework that is used to measure and analyze the patterns of individual and group level social interactions13. While it has found wide-ranging applications in animal behavior, only a handful of empirical studies to date have focused on calves14,15,16. Cattle form preferential social bonds and maintain affiliative relationships (e.g.,17,18). There are also indications that social relationships are important for calves. Research has shown that dairy calves benefit from group housing19 and exhibit social behaviors that are not found with individually housed calves20,21. For example, weaned dairy calves spend more time grooming a familiar companion compared with an unfamiliar one22. Further, studies of cattle in naturalistic settings suggest that young dairy calves rest within social groups (‘creches’) with calves of the same age54, indicating that social rest within same-age groups is an important natural behavior, with a speculated function of forming and reinforcing social bonds.

In this initial study, we quantified social networks for four groups of Holstein bull calves based on interaction data from a two-day experimental challenge that induced a component of bovine respiratory disease. Continuous behavioral data were recorded with digital video recording software. Social network position was measured using degree, strength and eigenvector centrality. We first establish whether social connections differ over the two-day challenge by comparing the consistency of dyadic social interactions and individual centrality scores. Next, to determine the possible effects of the experimental challenge on social network position, we examined individual centrality scores on challenge day and its relation to health status. Secondly, we tested whether an individual’s social network position differed significantly from baseline to challenge day and if this was related to the challenge. We hypothesize that the experimental challenge will result in reduced centrality and social connectedness for challenged calves, whereas control calves would maintain or increase connectedness with penmates.

Methods

Animal management

At the University of Florida Dairy Unit (Hague, FL, USA), Holstein bull calves (n = 24; aged 3 to 7 weeks) were placed into 4 groups based on age (6 calves/pen; 6.6 m2/calf) the week prior to the experiment. This was a convenience sample, with age range dependent upon the calf birth rate at the research facility. The age range within each pen was 7.5 ± 3.3 days (mean ± SE). This study was conducted prior to milk-weaning and calves were given pasteurized waste milk mixed with a powdered enhancer via teats, provided at a rate of 8 L/d in 2 daily meals (0600 and 1700 h). In addition, grain concentrate (via mounted buckets), and water were provided ad libitum.

Experimental design and disease challenge

Calves were blocked into pairs by age and body weight and randomly assigned within pen to 1 of 2 treatments: (1) a disease challenge involving inoculation at the tracheal bifurcation with 3 × 109 cfu of Mannheimia haemolytica (MH), a main component of bovine respiratory disease23, suspended in 5 ml of sterile phosphate buffered saline followed by a 120 ml wash (n = 12 calves), or (2) control involving inoculation with phosphate buffered saline only (n = 12 calves)24. Treatments were imposed when the mean calf age (across all 4 pens) was 35.9 ± 8.8 days (mean ± SD). This disease challenge model was validated in 8-week-old dairy calves25. Based upon this data, a sample size of 8 was determined to be sufficient in detecting differences in lying behavior, using a level of significance of 0.05 and 80% power. However, we increased the sample size to 12 calves/treatment to increase power to detect differences in social interactions, which may be subject to greater individual variability than lying time. Effects of this disease challenge model on clinical health outcomes as well as some individual behavioral responses of calves in the present study were described previously11. Briefly, the experimental challenge caused a mild disease state: rectal temperatures of challenged calves were elevated, differing at 12 h post-inoculation (40.1 vs. 39.1; standard error = 0.14), and some changes in behavior including differences in lying laterality and decreased frequency of lying bouts11. The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of the University of Florida approved and deemed all procedures and experiments listed in this study were ethical for animal care (#201408643). In addition, the research was conducted in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines, and all methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Behavioral observation

Video was recorded continuously throughout the trial (using Behavioral Observation Research Interactive Software; BORIS26). The average temperature during the study period was 25 degrees Celsius (min 23, max 31). Two 8 h. observational periods were conducted at 1100 h. on the day preceding treatment (baseline day; Day −1) and at 1100 h. immediately following inoculation (challenge day; Day 0). This window of observation was selected to evaluate the short-term acute response to the disease challenge, as previous data suggested that calf clinical signs of illness following this challenge were short-lived and returned to baseline within 24 h25. Individual calves were identified through coat markings and behavior (Table 1) was coded by one observer, who was blind to which calf underwent the disease challenge. Intra-observer reliability was calculated for one 8 h. period for 6 calves, with Cohen’s kappa = 0.94 as calculated in BORIS. The following behaviors were recorded continuously: frequency of social contact between individual calves and frequency and duration of social lying, with identity of all pairs of calves recorded. To further assess the direction of social interactions, initiated and received social contacts between calves were characterized on Day 0; these included all instances of directed behavior and excluded behaviors without a clear actor/recipient (such as side-to-side contact while passing). Social contacts and social lying were both observed to characterize different aspects of social behavior, including active social interactions, all physical social contact, and more passive social rest.

Table 1 Ethogram used to describe behaviors of calves (35.9 ± 8.8 days of age) housed in groups (6 calves/pen) exposed to either an experimental challenge model (inoculation with M. haemolytica) or a control (inoculation with sterile saline).

Social network data

Adjacency matrices were created from the frequency and duration of social interactions between calves in their respective pens. All networks had the same number of individual calves. A directed network was constructed from the frequency of all initiated social contacts, excluding any mutual contacts without clear actor/recipient on day of challenge only. Because this is a novel study aiming to investigate possible changes in social network position in response to an experimentally induced challenge, we opted to look at directed behaviors on challenge day only, since we were interested in the direction of affiliative behaviors. Additionally, three undirected networks were constructed with the frequency of social lying bouts, the duration of lying bouts, and the frequency of all social contacts on baseline day and day of challenge. We analyzed both the directed and undirected matrices as weighted networks to maintain the strength of the interactions, given our small per pen group size.

We examined direct and indirect social connections between the calves’ and their pen mates with three centrality measures at the individual or node level: degree, strength and eigenvector (reviewed in27). Nodal metrics describe each node’s position in the network relative to the other nodes28. These measures were chosen because they have proved useful in describing other dairy calves’ social networks14. Degree centrality examines the numbers of social connections that an individual node has. This measure indicates a node’s importance in the network and represents direct connections between individuals by measuring the number of edges that are connected to the node. The more nodes that a focal subject interacts with, the more central it is and the higher its degree centrality score. Strength, also known as weighted degree centrality, is a measure of the sum of weights assigned to the node's direct connections and represents the node strength13,29. Lastly, the eigenvector centrality is based on the sum of the centralities of an individual’s neighbors and the centrality of that node’s direct and indirect connections. For the directed networks of all-social contacts on challenge day, we also calculated the number of connections given and received (in-degree and out-degree) and the strength of these in-coming and out-going connections (in-strength and out-strength)30. Centrality measures were calculated using the igraph package in R (R Development Core Team, Version 4.1.131,32) .

Data analysis

To assess the effects of the disease challenge on social network position we examined both the directed and undirected networks. First, we generated separate linear mixed-effect models (LMMs) with the function “lmer” in the R package lme433. Social network metrics derived from directed interactions on challenge day were entered as the dependent variable. In all models, we included treatment (MH/CON) and age as fixed effects and pen number as a random factor. Calves were housed in pens according to their age (mean ± SD: 46.5 ± 1.52 days in pen 1, 41.0 ± 3.2 days in pen 2, 29.7 ± 5.1 days in pen 3, and 26.5 ± 1.87 days in pen 4); therefore, these two variables were significantly positively correlated (r = 0.871, df = 24, p = 0.01, Pearson correlation test). To analyze the effects of the disease challenge for the 3 undirected networks (social lying frequency, social lying duration, and all-social contacts frequency), we calculated the differences in individual centrality scores (degree, strength, eigenvector) from baseline (Day −1) to challenge (Day 0). These differences, represented as positive or negative values, were entered as the predictor in separate LMMs, with treatment (MH/CON) and age as fixed effects and pen number as a random effect.

For each LMM, model assumptions (i.e., residual normality distribution plots) were visually checked using “check_model” from the R package performance34. Four variables were log10 transformed to achieve a normal distribution (directed networks: in-strength and out-strength, undirected networks: social lying frequency eigenvector difference and social lying duration strength difference). We tested for multicollinearity of the independent factors by calculating the variance inflation factor “vif” in R package car35. There was no evidence of multicollinearity between factors (maximum variance inflation factor = 1.01). All analyses were performed in R with two-tailed tests and alpha level set at 0.0532.

To account for the relational nature of social network data, permutation-based regression tests (e.g., node label permutations) have been widely adopted36. Hart et al.37 found that node permutations can be replaced with parametric regression given a well-specified parametric model when centrality metrics are regressed against nodal covariates. Although our models met this criterion, given our calves were sampled equally through automated observational methods and were restricted to their respective group-based pens, node permutations should also perform well38.

Our null hypothesis procedure consisted of building 1000 random networks using node label permutations on our social network centrality metrics (“perm.net.nl”), package ANTs in R39. Our null model maintained the variance structure in terms of network positions and thus the same distribution of node values in each social network. We shuffled SNA centrality metric data between individuals within their respective pen networks after the network had been inferred. We then ran our original LMMs with the permuted statistics (1000 permutations of each SNA metric) with a confidence interval set at 95% to obtain unbiased significance tests for the coefficients40.

Lastly, in order to explore the similarity of the three networks (social lying frequency, social lying duration and all-social contacts), we used a multiple regression quadratic assignment procedure with double semipartialling (MRQAP-DSP) and 1000 permutations in the sna R package41. We compared the duration and frequency matrices of dyadic lying behavior and whether social lying is related to all social contacts. MRQAP controls for autocorrelations in matrix regressions by using a Monte Carlo method of permutation to test significance42,43. For these models we only used undirected data from baseline day in order to minimize possible effects from the challenge.

Results

MH challenged calves less connected in directed networks on challenge day

We found strong evidence that the experimental challenge influenced the calves’ social network position on challenge day; challenged calves interacted with fewer penmates overall (lower degree centrality), spent less time in contact with others (lower strength centrality), and were less connected to more central individuals (lower eigenvector centrality) (Table 2; Fig. 1). Overall, control calves were more central and well-connected in their directed social contact network than would be expected by chance (p < 0.01 for eigenvector, degree and strength). Further, there was a significant, negative relationship between the experimental challenge and in-degree, in-strength network measures and a positive interaction between the challenge and out-degree. In other words, challenged calves initiated social contact with more penmates on challenge day (higher out-degree), while control calves received more social interactions from more penmates (higher in-strength/in-degree). Age was also associated with degree, out-degree and eigenvector centrality (Table 2).

Table 2 Results of linear mixed-effect models for directed networks of bull calves (35.9 ± 8.8 days of age) housed in groups (6 calves/pen): centrality metrics, testing the main fixed effects of calves exposed to an experimental challenge model (inoculation with M. haemolytica) or a sham procedure (inoculation with sterile saline) and age, random effect (Pen): coefficient estimates, standard errors, t-test and p-values.
Figure 1
figure 1

Diagnostic box plots represent the relationship between social network measures and an experimental challenge model (inoculation with M. haemolytica) or a sham procedure (inoculation with sterile saline) of bull calves (35.9 ± 8.8 d of age) housed in groups (6 calves/pen). Each plot shows the mean and distribution centrality network metrics (a) strength centrality, (b) degree centrality, (c) eigenvector centrality plotted for “challenged” (MH) and “healthy” (CON) individuals in each Pen (X-axis). Figure was created using R32.

Reduced centrality in undirected networks for MH challenged calves during the challenge

We calculated the change in network position to determine whether shifts in calves’ social connectedness were related to health. The difference in individual centrality scores, based on undirected interactions on baseline (Day -1) and challenge day (Day 0), were related to the experimental challenge for some measures (Table 3). In the all-social contacts network, the difference in scores for all three centrality measures were related to the challenge (p < 0.01 for eigenvector, degree and strength) (Table 3). Compared to control calves, challenged calves had a decrease in centrality and reduction in connectedness for all social contacts (Fig. 2). Conversely, control calves had an increase in the strength of social lying frequency connections. Changes in degree and eigenvector centrality for the frequency of social lying were not related to the challenge. Nor did we find relationships between the experimental challenge and changes in centrality for any measures of social lying duration.

Table 3 Results of linear mixed-effect models for undirected networks of bull calves (35.9 ± 8.8 days of age) housed in groups (6 calves/pen): differences in centrality measures between baseline and challenge day, testing the main fixed effects of calves exposed to an experimental challenge model (inoculation with M. haemolytica) or a sham procedure (inoculation with sterile saline) and age, random effect (Pen): coefficient estimates, standard errors, t-test and p-values.
Figure 2
figure 2

Network representation of the ‘All-Social Contacts Frequency’ networks of group-housed (n = 6/pen) bull calves (35.9 ± 8.8 d of age) exposed to an experimental challenge model (inoculation with M. haemolytica) or a sham procedure (inoculation with sterile saline): (a) baseline day: Pen#1–Pen#4, versus (b) challenge day: Pen#1–Pen#4. Individual calves (nodes) are represented by the circles; size is based on strength centrality; lines represent the weighted interactions among penmates. Baseline centrality scores were significantly different from centrality scores on challenge day and these differences were related to health. Figure was created using R32.

Dyadic interactions differ for active social behavior and social rest

We evaluated the similarity of the duration and frequency of dyadic lying behavior during the baseline period and whether it is related to all social contacts among calves, to explore whether the three interaction networks show meaningful differences and if some measures are more useful in determining sickness behavior. We found that social lying frequency was a strong predictor of lying duration for all four of the pens on baseline day (MRQAP, pen1: r = 0.42, p = 0.01; pen2: r = 0.77, p < 0.001; pen3: r = 0.78, p < 0.001; pen4: r = 0.83, p < 0.001). Notably, neither lying frequency nor duration predicted affiliative interactions in the all-social contact networks. This suggests that calves’ affiliative interactions are different for active social interactions (e.g., grooming and head butting), compared to more passive social rest.

Discussion

Our study assessed the effects of an experimental challenge on the social networks of group-housed dairy calves. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine individual centrality (connectedness within the social network) of dairy calves following experimental challenge of a respiratory pathogen. As predicted, we found challenged calves (compared to controls) were less central in their directed social contact networks on challenge day. In addition, challenged calves but not controls, had reduced centralities for all three measures in the undirected social contact network, baseline to challenge day. These results are consistent with previous work on social network position which found that immune-challenged bats injected with lipopolysaccharide had lower individual centrality scores in association networks, compared to healthy bats2. Further, it adds to a growing body of evidence that sickness behavior44,45 includes a reduction in social connections and social preference with conspecifics and is indicative of illness in a variety of species (e.g., mammals:46, fish:47, insects:48). In contrast, control calves had on average higher levels of centrality for directed social contact networks, and an increase in strength centrality for undirected social lying frequency networks, from baseline to challenge day. This suggests healthy calves did not avoid challenged penmates, and instead increased social lying connectedness. Although it may be advantageous to avoid infectious conspecifics49, increased social interaction with sick penmates has also been reported in group-housed pigs50,51.

Illness and inflammation in humans have been linked with an increase in approach behavior towards close contacts7, who may offer support and care to aid in recovery52. These findings lend support to the effect we found on higher social contact out-degree for challenged calves. It may be that initiating social contact is more beneficial than receiving social interactions when an individual is sick. Grooming is an important affiliative behavior that primarily functions to maintain social bonds in many social species (e.g.53), including calves15, (reviewed in54). In nonhuman primates, giving rather than receiving grooming has been shown to mitigate social stress and stress-related hormone levels (Macaca sylvanus:55; Papio cynocephalus ursinus:56). While animals housed in pens cannot completely withdraw from infected group members, our results indicate calves exhibit more complex sickness behavior by increasing some social interactions with familiar peers, similar to that seen in humans and nonhuman primates (reviewed in8,10).

There is considerable interest in measuring behavior as an indicator of disease in dairy calves, given the long-term animal welfare consequences. In particular, bovine respiratory disease is the second leading cause of early life mortality in preweaned dairy calves57. Previous work has evaluated how changes in behavior may coincide with respiratory disease in dairy calves, either naturally occurring or resulting from an experimental disease challenge. For example, disease has been associated with changes in patterns of milk feeding (reviewed in58) and lying behavior (reviewed in59). Calves with naturally occurring respiratory disease have shown a decrease in the frequency of lying bouts (e.g.60) and a reduction in milk feeding behaviors (e.g.61), compared to healthy calves. Our study expands on these findings and suggests that in addition to changes in feeding and activity behavior, calves modify their social interactions in response to experimentally induced illness.

We examined social rest given evidence that this may be a meaningful indicator of social bonding62. In group-housed dairy calves, greater durations of social rest were previously observed between familiar calves, compared to unfamiliar calves21. While an increase in lying time60 and the frequency of lying bouts63 have been associated with naturally occurring respiratory disease, we found no evidence that changes in individual centrality for social lying duration were associated with sickness. Nor did we find relationships between the changes in individual degree and eigenvector centrality for social lying frequency and the disease challenge for either challenged or control calves. Calf space allowance was somewhat greater than industry standard for dairy calves (e.g. 4.6 m2/calf:64) in this study, but not outside the range of observed values. Low space allowance has been linked to a reduction in play behavior in diary calves65, while an increase in allowance is associated with reduced lying behavior66. However, to our knowledge, research on the effects of space allowance and the expression of social behavior has only been conducted on healthy individuals.

In order to evaluate the potential use of changes in relationship between social network position and its relation to health, it is important to distinguish between networks of discrete behaviors. We found that the social lying frequency network was a strong predictor of the social lying duration network. However, neither social lying frequency nor duration predicted the all-social contact network. These results suggest that network measures of directed affiliative interactions (e.g. social grooming and head butting) are different than network measures of passive social rest (e.g. frequency and duration of social lying bouts). Thus, networks constructed with active and passive affiliative interactions may be useful in understanding changes in social interactions and sickness behavior.

Conclusion

Here, we provide evidence that an experimental challenge influenced the social networks of group-housed dairy calves. Compared to control calves, challenged calves had reduced centrality and social connectedness over the two-day challenge and less central network positions on challenge day. Additionally, challenged calves initiated contact with more penmates on challenge day, potentially functioning to elicit social support. In addition to previous findings that focused on initiated dyadic social interactions, this is the first study demonstrating that changes in social network position coincide with an experimentally induced challenge in calves.