Introduction

While animal cooperation typically occurs among related individuals1, there are cases of unrelated cooperators in some mammal, bird and insect species2. These have raised interesting questions on the origin, maintenance and benefits of cooperation among unrelated animals. Such questions include how the internal (e.g., physiology) and external (e.g., environment) conditions affect the decision to behave cooperatively, the benefits of cooperation, and its outcome. While social insects are typical model systems for kin-based cooperation, the early stage of the colony life of some ant species provides a classic example of cooperation among unrelated partners2.

Pleometrosis in ants is the foundation of colonies by two or more cooperating newly-mated queens that settle in the same nest site after a nuptial flight3,4,5,6. In some cases, pleometrosis results in mature multiple-queen colonies, as in pleometrotic populations of the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus7,8. However, in most species, pleometrotic associations are only transitory: the queens stay together to produce eggs, but upon worker emergence, queens engage in fights (sometimes initiated and/or joined by the workers) until a single queen survives4. Pleometrosis results in an earlier production of larger groups of workers compared to solitary foundation, which may be beneficial in dense populations where young colonies compete for food and brood raiding can be common3,7,9,10,11,12. Whether pleometrosis merely increases egg production or also provides additional benefits in term of brood development or immunity remains unclear13,14.

The queens that do not survive the pleometrotic associations have no fitness benefits, as they die before producing sexuals (the fertile individuals that are only produced in mature colonies). Thus, understanding the factors that determine which queens survive the associations would allow researchers to draw and test hypotheses on whether or not queens with given characteristics should decide to cooperate with other queens, as well as interpret previous reports of such queen decisions3,15.

Several determinants of queen survival in pleometrotic associations have been identified. Surviving queens tend to be larger16,17,18,19, heavier15,18, to spend more time on the brood pile20,21, and to lose less weight during colony foundation17,20. However, these reports may have been subject to confounding effects because the factors tested are usually interconnected. For example, body size and mass may correlate with fecundity16, which in turn influences the number of workers produced and therefore the distribution of parentages in the offspring (although there is no evidence that workers favour their own mother16,17,20). One strategy to control for such confounded factors is to experimentally pair queens that differ in one factor but not in others, thus allowing the identification of factors that affect queen survival irrespective of others.

In this study, we used the black garden ant Lasius niger to study cooperation among unrelated individuals in pleometrotic associations. We first quantified the benefits of pleometrosis in terms of brood production and development. Then we tested whether queen fecundity and body size, two factors that may be associated with queen survival16,17,18,20 were correlated. We investigated the effect of fecundity and size on queen survival in experimental pleometrotic associations, in which paired queens differed in one trait but not the other, while controlling for worker parentage. Finally, we compared fecundity and size between queens that were selected as pleometrotic partners and queens that were not.

Results

Effect of pleometrosis on offspring production

To investigate the effect of pleometrosis on offspring production, we set up experimental colonies with one or two L. niger founding queens. Our comparisons of the number of eggs, larvae, pupae and workers produced in these experimental colonies revealed benefits of pleometrosis (Fig. 1A). We found that pleometrosis increased the maximum number of eggs (ANOVA: F1, 25 = 39.18, P < 0.00001; Fig. 1B) and pupae (ANOVA: F1, 25 = 6.93, P = 0.014; Fig. 1D) recorded in the colonies, as well as the number of workers produced (ANOVA: F1, 20 = 16.62, P = 0.0006; Fig. 1E). However, our analysis did not detect such an effect for the maximum number of larvae (ANOVA: F1, 25 = 2.25, P = 0.15; Fig. 1C).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of queen number on (A) mean offspring number over time since colony foundation (colors denote the brood type, while line type indicates treatment), maximum number of (B) eggs, (C) larvae, (D) pupae recorded in the colonies, and (E) number of workers at the end of the experiment (mean ± standard error).

The benefits of pleometrosis disappeared when correcting for the number of queens present in the colonies. The per capita numbers of eggs and larvae were actually reduced in colonies with two queens (ANOVA for eggs: F1, 25 = 3.97, P = 0.057; ANOVA for larvae: F1, 25 = 10.94, P = 0.0029), and we found no effect of pleometrosis on the numbers of pupae and workers per queen (ANOVA for pupae: F1, 25 = 1.60, P = 0.22; ANOVA for workers: F1, 20 = 0.43, P = 0.52).

We did not detect any effect of queen number on the time to produce the first egg (Wilcoxon test: W = 107, P = 0.19; Fig. 2A) and larva (Wilcoxon test: W = 103, P = 0.11; Fig. 2B), but colonies with two queens were significantly faster than those with one queen in producing their first pupa (Wilcoxon test: W = 114.5, P = 0.01; Fig. 2C) and worker (Wilcoxon test: W = 111, P = 0.011; Fig. 2D).

Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of queen number on the day (mean ± s.e.) when the first (A) eggs, (B) larvae, (C) pupae, and (D) workers were recorded in the colonies.

Finally, once the first workers emerged, colonies with two queens showed a faster increase in worker number than colonies with a single queen (ANOVA: F1,171.4 = 44.6, P < 0.00001; Fig. 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

The number of workers increased faster in colonies with two queens (crosses, dotted regression line, slope = 1.01, R2 = 0.77) compared to those with one queen (squares, solid regression line, slope = 0.59, R2 = 0.53) (ANOVA: F1,171.4 = 44.6, P < 0.00001).

Association between fecundity and size

To identify the factors that influence queen survival in pleometrotic associations requires the decoupling of candidate variables, such as fecundity and size. We found a weak but significant, positive correlation between thorax length and brood number in L. niger founding queens (ANOVA: adjusted R2 = 0.034, F1, 229 = 9.13, P = 0.0028). This result confirms that the effects of size and fecundity can be confounded, but suggests that it is possible to disentangle them experimentally.

Effect of fecundity and size on queen survival

To investigate the effect of fecundity and size on queen survival, we produced experimental pairs of L. niger queens that differed in one factor, but not the other. Among the 35 pairs of queens that differed in fecundity (but not size), seven pairs still had two queens at the end of the experiment. Out of the remaining 28 pairs that lost a queen, the most fecund queen survived in 75% (21 out of 28) of the cases, which represented a significant departure from 50% (exact binomial test, P = 0.012; Fig. 4A). Among the 33 pairs of queens that differed in size (but not fecundity), five pairs still had two queens at the end of the experiment, and in one additional case, the two queens died on the same day. Out of the remaining 27 pairs that lost a queen, the largest queen survived in 63% (17 out of 27) of the cases, which did not differ significantly from 50% (exact binomial test, P = 0.25; Fig. 4B).

Figure 4
figure 4

Likelihood of surviving the pleometrotic association depending on queen (A) fecundity (n = 28) and (B) size (n = 27). P values come from binomial tests.

Difference between chosen and not chosen queens in fecundity and size

To study the traits associated with partner selection in pleometrotic L. niger queens, we provided founding queens with the choice between two, randomly selected queens. Then we compared the brood production and size of chosen and not chosen queens. We found that the maximum number of eggs recorded was lower for queens that were previously chosen as pleometrotic partners compared to queens that were not chosen (ANOVA: χ2 = 4.56, P = 0.033; Fig. 5A). We could not find such difference for the maximum number of larvae (ANOVA: χ2 = 0.41, P = 0.52) and pupae (ANOVA: χ2 = 0, P = 1). In addition, we could not detect any difference in thorax length between chosen and not chosen queens (ANOVA: χ2 = 0.66, P = 0.41; Fig. 5B).

Figure 5
figure 5

Difference between queens that were chosen and not chosen in the choice tests in (A) maximum number of eggs recorded and (B) thorax length (mean ± s.e.).

Discussion

In this study, we used the black garden ant Lasius niger to investigate the benefits and factors of pleometrosis, the transitory association between founding queens. The monitoring of colonies founded by one or two queens showed that pleometrosis increased and accelerated offspring production. Then, the experimental pairing of L. niger founding queens revealed that in pairs of queens of different fecundity but similar size, the most fecund queen was more likely to survive. Our experiment could not detect a similar effect of size when controlling for fecundity. Finally, we found that queens associated preferentially with less fecund queens.

Our findings of pleometrosis benefiting offspring production are in line with the literature for this, and other ant species3,7,9,10,12,22,23. Interestingly, we only detected these benefits at the colony level, as pleometrosis had either no effect or a negative influence on the per capita offspring production9,12,22. However, colony-level measurements are more relevant in the case of pleometrosis, as the queen that survives the association inherits all the offspring produced during colony foundation. In the field, colonies with a faster, more efficient worker production would have a competitive advantage over neighbouring founding colonies3,4. This is especially true for L. niger, which shows high density of founding colonies that compete for limiting resources and raid the brood of other colonies10. Thus, the competitive advantage provided by pleometrosis likely enhances colony growth and survival.

The increased and faster production of workers in colonies with two queens may stem from a nutritional boost for the larvae. L. niger founding queens do not forage, and produce the first cohort of workers from their own metabolic reserves. Larvae have been observed to cannibalize both viable and non-viable (trophic) eggs24. We found that colonies with two queens produced more eggs, but that this did not translate in them having more larvae. However, more of these larvae became pupae—and ultimately workers. In addition, while the time to produce the first egg and larva did not differ between colonies with one and two queens, the first pupa and worker were produced faster when two queens were present, consistent with a shorter larval stage. We propose that larvae in pleometrotic colonies developed faster and were more likely to reach pupation because they had more eggs that provided nutrients, boosting the development rate of the first workers.

These benefits of pleometrosis are only inherited by the queens that survive, it is thus important to understand the factors that determine queen survival in pleometrotic associations. Although this question has been relatively well studied3,16,17,18,19,20,21, it has remained challenging to disentangle the effects of correlated factors. For example, we found that size, which has been reported to predict queen survival16,19, correlated with fecundity, which would itself be confounded with the parentage of workers in the first cohort produced. To address this issue, we disentangled size and fecundity experimentally, and used foreign workers that developed from pupae collected in field colonies to prevent any potential nepotistic behaviour.

We found that fecundity, but not size, determined queen survival. The finding that, despite being of similar size, more fecund queens are more likely to survive indicates that the outcome of pleometrosis is not the mere consequence of physical dominance. The higher fecundity could reflect a better health condition, which may give the advantage to the more fecund queen in direct fights3,15, or if workers initiate the fights. Natural selection may have favoured workers that skew aggression toward the less fecund queen, both because this queen would be less efficient at building a colony, and because the workers would be more likely to be the offspring of the more fecund queen. The latter would not necessarily involve direct nepotistic behaviours (the workers would not behave according to parentage, but to fecundity), which have remained elusive in social insects in general25,26,27, and in pleometrotic associations in particular16,17,20. Despite regular behavioural observations, we did not observe who initiated aggression in our experiments, and it remains unclear whether the queens and/or the workers are responsible for the onset of fights. Consistently with previous studies16,23, we found that a certain proportion of queen death occurred before worker emergence, suggesting that worker presence is not required for queen execution. Finally, we cannot rule out that the least fecund queens were more likely to die because of a weaker health status, possibly combined with the stress of being associated with another, healthier queen.

Although it has not been directly reported before, our finding that fecundity determines queen survival is consistent with previous reports of weight being associated with queen survival17, more fecund queens being more aggressive28, cuticular hydrocarbon profiles differing between surviving and culled queens21, and between more and less fecund queens28. We could not directly support previous reports of size correlating with survival16,19. This could be because in those studies, size could have been confounded with fecundity, and/or because we lacked the statistical power to detect such effect in our experiment.

Pleometrosis provides clear benefits, but these benefits are only inherited by the surviving queens, and the losing queens pay the great cost of dying without contributing to the next generation. Natural selection should thus favour queens that decide whether or not to join a pleometrotic association based on the relative benefits compared to individual foundation—these may differ across ecological contexts29—and the likelihood of surviving the association. As fecundity appears to determine queen survival in L. niger, queens may have evolved the ability to choose among potential partners according to their fecundity. Our results are consistent with this hypothesis, as queens preferentially associated with partners that would later produce fewer eggs, possibly because they were less fecund, and therefore less healthy and easier to eliminate. This suggests that founding queens may assess the fecundity of potential partners, possibly via their cuticular hydrocarbon profile28. This result further supports our finding that fecundity plays an important role in pleometrotic associations. It is important to note that this difference in egg production could have alternative explanations. First, it could stem from more fecund queens having no interest in forming an association because they are able to start a competitive colony alone. Second, it could be a consequence, rather than a cause, of the outcome of the choice experiment. We cannot rule out that entering an association with another queen and/or leaving this association prematurely at the end of the choice experiment may have been stressful for the chosen queens, and affected their later production of eggs. We could not detect any difference between chosen and not chosen queens in the number of larvae and pupae produced, which are likely influenced by factors other than fecundity (e.g., brood care behaviour). Interestingly, we did not find that queens chose according to size, consistent with our finding that size may not affect which queen survives the pleometrotic association.

Our study informs on the benefits and factors of pleometrosis, and highlights the role of fecundity in the decision to associate with another queen, and in determining which queen survives the association. As such, it contributes to a better understanding of the onset and outcome of pleometrosis, a classic case of cooperation between unrelated animals.

Methods

Sample collection

We used L. niger as study organism because queens in this species commonly found new colonies in pleometrotic associations that consist of typically unrelated individuals3. We collected 411 L. niger founding queens in Mainz, Germany right after nuptial flights on June 26th 2017 (cohort 1, 138 queens), July 9th 2017 (cohort 2, 138 queens) and July 4th 2020 (cohort 3, 135 queens). All founding queens had shed their wings at the time of collection. The queens were housed individually in glass test tubes (10 × 1.2 cm) with water blocked by a cotton ball, and then kept in the dark at 21 °C.

Experimental designs

We randomly selected 45 queens from cohort 2 to investigate the benefits of pleometrosis. Half the test tubes contained a single queen (n = 15), and the other half contained two queens (n = 15). Then we recorded the number of eggs, larvae, pupae and workers three times a week for 53 days. Upon emergence of the first workers, we placed the tubes in larger plastic boxes (11 × 15 × 3 cm) and fed the colonies once a week with a mixture of honey, eggs, agar and water30. Eight queens died over the course of the experiment: three queens in the single-queen treatment died four, 11 and 16 days after set-up, and were removed from the analyses. Five queens in the two-queen treatment died 37, 42 (two associations) and 49 (two associations) days after set-up. In each of those five cases, one queen of the two queens survived. These tubes were kept in the analyses because queen execution is expected in pleometrotic associations.

We used 231 queens (138 from cohort 1, 93 from cohort 2) to investigate the effect of size and fecundity on queen survival. On day 17 after the nuptial flights, we recorded the number of brood items (eggs and larvae) in each tube, and used this measure as a proxy for fecundity, thus assuming that egg production changes similarly over time across queens. The number of brood items equalled the number of eggs for most of the queens (152/231), as they had only produced eggs at this point. On days 19 and 20 after the nuptial flights, we used a Leica stereomicroscope to measure the thorax length of the founding queens, and used it as a proxy for size in all subsequent analyses.

To test how difference in fecundity between queens affects the outcome of the pleometrotic association, we experimentally paired 70 paint-marked queens (35 pairs) that differed in the number of brood items (high fecundity: 65.5 ± 7.8, mean ± sd; low fecundity: 30.5 ± 11.6; one sample t-test against zero, t = − 15.1, df = 34, P < 0.0001), but not in thorax length (high fecundity: 3.2 mm ± 0.4; low fecundity: 3.2 mm ± 0.4; one sample t-test against zero, t = 0.59, df = 34, P = 0.56). To do so, we paired queens that had a high number of brood items (top 25% of the distribution) with queens that had a low number of brood items (bottom 25% of the distribution, excluding the queens that did not produce any eggs), while making sure that they had a medium size (both in the middle 50% of the distribution). We used a similar strategy to investigate how size difference between queens affects their survival, and paired 66 paint-marked queens (33 pairs) that differed in size (large: 3.2 mm ± 0.4; small: 2.8 mm ± 0.3; one sample t-test against zero, t =  − 18.15, df = 32, P < 0.0001) but not in fecundity (large: 49.4 ± 8.9; small: 49.2 ± 9.7; one sample t-test against zero, t =  − 0.36, df = 32, P = 0.72). Before being paired, each queen was marked with a color dot on the thorax using a toothpick dipped into Edding marker paint. Although there is no evidence that workers favour their own mother in pleometrotic associations16,17,20, we wanted to prevent worker parentage from being confounded with queen fecundity. Thus, we did not provide the queen pairs with their own brood, but with 25 pupae from a pool of pupae collected in field colonies around Mainz, Germany.

We used 135 queens (cohort 3) to investigate how fecundity and size correlate with the likelihood of being chosen as a pleometrotic partner. To do so, we first performed choice test experiments on the three days following the nuptial flight. We used test arenas that consisted of two plastic tubes (length: 4 cm) covered with red foil, and connected them on opposite sides to a plastic petri dish (diameter: 5 cm). We tethered a queen to each tube by attaching one end of a metal wire (length: ca. 1 cm; diameter: 0.02 mm) to its petiole and the other end to the bottom of the tube. We then introduced a choosing queen in the central petri dish, which could move freely into both tubes, and interact with the queens attached to the bottom of each tube. This set-up provided the choosing queen with two possible pleometrotic partners. We used Sony FDR-AX33 video cameras to record the arenas for 12 h after introduction of the choosing queen. We tested 45 groups of three queens. For each test, we randomly selected one of the three queens to be the choosing queen. We used a set of three criteria to consider that a choosing queen had made a choice: (1) it should be observed inside one of the two tubes at the end of the video (12 h after its introduction to the arena), (2) it should not have left the tube for at least one hour before the end of the video, and (3) it should be observed in the same tube the next day (24 h after its introduction to the arena). In 27 choice tests (out of 45), no decision was made because the choosing queen did not settle in one of the two tubes, or because one of the choice queens escaped their metal wire. Thus in the remaining 18 tests, the choosing queen made a decision, which provided us with 18 chosen queens and 18 not chosen queens. These queens were then kept individually at 21 °C, and monitored once a week to count the number of eggs, larvae and pupae present in their tube. The monitoring stopped when the first worker emerged or 80 days after the choice test (12 queens did not produce workers by that time). We used a Leica stereomicroscope to measure the thorax length of the queens at the end of the experiment. Two queens died during monitoring, and were removed from the analysis, as well as the other queens from the same test arena, resulting in 16 chosen and 16 not chosen queens in the analysis.

Statistics

To investigate the effect of the number of queens on absolute and per capita offspring production, we have extracted for each colony the highest number of brood items recorded for each brood type, as well as the number of workers at the end of the experiment, and compared those values between single-queen and two-queen colonies using type II ANOVAs on linear models. To investigate the effect of queen number on the timing of offspring production and development, we compared the days when the first egg, larva, pupa and worker were recorded between colonies with one and two queens using non-parametric Wilcoxon tests. To further investigate the effect of queen number on worker production, we focused on the days after the first workers were produced, and conducted a type II ANOVA on a mixed-effect linear model with the number of workers as response variable, time and number of queens as fixed response variable, and queen identity as a random effect. To investigate the association between size and fecundity in L. niger founding queens, we conducted a type II ANOVA on a linear model that explained the number of brood items with thorax length (values were standardized within queen collection cohorts for both measurements). To test whether size and fecundity determined queen survival, we compared proportion of surviving queens to 50% using exact binomial tests. To investigate whether chosen and not chosen queens differed in brood production, we extracted for each queen the maximum number of brood items recorded for each brood type, and conducted a type II ANOVA on a mixed-effect linear model with the maximum number of brood items as response variable, queen category as a fixed response variable, and test arena as a random effect. We performed the same analysis to test the effect of queen category on thorax length. All linear models built in this study were checked for normal distribution of the residuals.